Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T02:35:44.580Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Introduction of iodised salt benefits infants’ mental development in a community-based cluster-randomised effectiveness trial in Ethiopia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 March 2018

Karim Bougma*
Affiliation:
School of Human Nutrition, McGill University, 21111 Lakeshore Road, CINEBuilding, Sainte Anne-de-Bellevue, QC, H9X 3V9, Canada
Frances E. Aboud
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, McGill University, 2001 McGill College Avenue, Montreal, QC, H3A 1G1, Canada
Tizita M. Lemma
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Bahir Dar University, PO Box 79, Bahir Dar, Ethiopia
Edward A. Frongillo
Affiliation:
Department of Health Promotion, Education, and Behavior, University of South Carolina, Discovery I, 915 Greene Street, Columbia, SC 29208, USA
Grace S. Marquis
Affiliation:
School of Human Nutrition, McGill University, 21111 Lakeshore Road, CINEBuilding, Sainte Anne-de-Bellevue, QC, H9X 3V9, Canada
*
*Corresponding author: K. Bougma, email [email protected], [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The effectiveness of salt iodisation in improving the mental development of young children has not been assessed. We implemented a community-based cluster-randomised effectiveness trial in sixty randomly selected districts in the Amhara region of Ethiopia. We randomly allocated each district to treatment and randomly selected one of its villages. In parallel to national salt iodisation efforts, iodised salt was brought early into the markets of the thirty intervention villages before it became widely available in the thirty control villages 4–6 months later. The primary outcome was children’s mental development scores on the Bayley Scales. This was an intention-to-treat analysis using mixed linear models adjusted for covariates and clusters. The trial was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT013496. We assessed 1835 infants aged 5–11 months at baseline. The same children (85 % of the sample) were re-assessed at 20–29 months when all villages had iodised salt. At endline, urinary iodine concentration was higher in children in the intervention group compared with those in the control group (median 228·0 v. 155·1 µg/l, P=0·001). The intervention group had higher scores compared with the control group on the Bayley composite score (raw scores:130·60 v. 128·51; standardised scores: 27·8 v. 26·9; d=0·13; 95 % CI 0·02, 0·23) and three of the four subscales: cognitive (53·27 v. 52·54, d=0·13; 95 % CI 0·03, 0·23), receptive language (20·71 v. 20·18, d=0·13; 95 % CI 0·03, 0·24) and fine motor (35·45 v. 34·94, d=0·15; 95 % CI 0·04, 0·25). The introduction of iodised salt contributes to children’s higher urinary iodine concentration and mental development.

Type
Full Papers
Copyright
Copyright © The Authors 2018 

About 43 % of children under 5 years of age in low- and middle-income countries do not reach their full developmental potential mainly because of inadequate psychosocial stimulation and nutritional deficiencies including iodine deficiency( Reference Black, Walker and Fernald 1 ). Iodine is required for the production of thyroid hormones, which are involved in brain development during fetal life and early infancy( Reference Berbel, Auso and Garcia-Velasco 2 , Reference Zoeller and Rovet 3 ). The consequences of iodine deficiency are said to include delayed motor and mental development, with cretinism being the most extreme form( Reference Delange 4 ).

Evidence for the effects of iodine on mental development comes mainly from children whose pregnant mothers were supplemented and from studies with school-aged children. A recent meta-analysis on the former that focused on development outcomes of children under 5 years of age found medium effect sizes of 0·48 for randomised controlled trials (RCT) and 0·46 for non-RCT( Reference Bougma, Aboud and Harding 5 ). The two RCT supplemented pregnant women with iodised oil capsules( Reference Pretell, Torres and Zenteno 6 , Reference Thilly 7 ). The results were limited by high attrition, small sample sizes and non-adjustment for cluster design in the analysis. The eight non-RCT had potential biases associated with uncontrolled confounders( Reference Connolly, Pharoah and Hetzel 8 Reference Gordon, Rose and Skeaff 13 ). Most relevant to the present study are two non-RCT iodine intervention trials that supplemented directly children from birth to 12 months and found effect sizes for mental development when tested at 2–5 years ranging from −0·31 to 0·33( Reference Cao, Jiang and Dou 14 , Reference Trowbridge 15 ). Consequently, there is little evidence regarding the effects of iodine given to children in the first 2 years of life, and no published RCT that assessed effects of iodised salt on mental development at this age( Reference Melse-Boonstra and Jaiswal 16 ).

The present study was designed to examine the effects of introducing iodised salt on young children’s mental development in Ethiopia where iodine deficiency ranked highest among African countries( Reference Andersson, Karumbunathan and Zimmermann 17 ). In 2005, only 4·2 % of households tested were using iodised salt( Reference Abuye, Berhane and Akalu 18 ), although one publication reported that 20 % of the households were using adequately iodised salt (≥15 parts per million (ppm))( 19 ); there was a 39·9 % prevalence of goitre and a median urinary iodine concentration (UIC) of 24·5 µg/l among children aged 6 to 12 years( Reference Abuye, Berhane and Akalu 18 ), indicating moderate to severe iodine deficiency( 20 ). The Government of Ethiopia passed universal salt iodisation (USI) legislation in 2011. As it was expected that iodised salt would gradually enter all markets as national production increased, this situation presented a unique opportunity to randomly assign early market introduction and assess the effect of iodised salt on young children’s mental development. The study hypothesis was that mental development scores would be higher in young children in intervention communities exposed to iodised salt earlier in comparison with young children from control communities who received it later.

Methods

The reporting of this cluster-randomised trial follows the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials extended guidelines( Reference Campbell, Piaggio and Elbourne 21 ).

Study design and participants

The study was a cluster-randomised effectiveness trial with district as the cluster that was the unit of randomisation. Randomisation of district clusters allowed for the distribution of salt to markets rather than to households or individuals. Within districts, one village was randomly selected to minimise contamination across treatment groups. Baseline and endline assessments were conducted to compare outcomes before and after the introduction of iodised salt.

Ethics approval was received from the Ethiopian Public Health Institute, McGill University and the National Research Ethics Review Committee (Ethiopia). Support letters were also obtained from Regional, Zonal and District Bureaus of Health. All mothers provided written informed consent. The trial was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT013496.

This study was carried out between 25 October 2010 and 13 October 2013 in sixty rural villages in Amhara, a northern region of Ethiopia. The Amhara region is divided into ten zones, each sub-divided into districts (woredas) and then into villages (kebeles). Of the ten Amhara zones, four were excluded for reasons of language, urbanisation, presence of community-based medical school training and use of alternative salt distribution. The remaining six contiguous zones served as the sampling frame.

A study census team visited households in selected villages to enumerate all infants who would be aged 5–11 months during baseline data collection. Enrolment and baseline data collection were performed simultaneously. Infants whose mothers were under 18 years of age were excluded.

Randomisation, allocation and masking

From the seventy-five eligible districts in the six zones, sixty were randomly selected using a simple randomisation method. Next, thirty districts were randomly allocated to the intervention arm and the remaining thirty to the control arm. In all sixty districts, one village was randomly selected for data collection.

Only the principal investigators and the person monitoring salt markets were aware of the group allocation. The research assistants were not told of the treatment allocation; they were present in the field only at baseline and endline when markets uniformly had non-iodised and iodised salt, respectively. Participants were also unaware of their condition; they continued to buy salt from their usual market, and when asked during the endline survey 88·1 % of mothers said they had not heard about iodised salt and 96·8 % said they never bought it, although 91·6 % of household salt was iodised. The iodisation process did not change the texture of the salt and the price was the same.

Procedures for intervention delivery and monitoring

The intervention consisted of introducing iodised salt to markets of the intervention villages in July 2012, ahead of its widespread distribution. The salt was harvested by national producers who began to iodise in sizeable amounts after March 2012. Once production began, but before enough was available to cover the country’s needs, we worked with the two wholesalers in the Amhara region to bring iodised salt to the district markets used by intervention villagers. In the thirty control villages, iodised salt became gradually available through regular market channels once production was sufficient and legislation was enforced in October 2012. In July 2012, 70·4 % of households in the intervention arm were using iodised salt; this increased to 82·8 % in August and to 89·5 % in September, whereas in October only 66·3 % of households in the control villages were using iodised salt. Children in the intervention group received approximately 4 to 6 months more exposure to iodised salt than controls; thus, at endline, children in the intervention group had approximately 8 to 10 months of exposure to iodised salt, whereas children in the control group had 4 to 6 months of exposure.

Outcome measures

All surveys were translated and back-translated. Measures were field-tested in one village that was not part of the sample.

Primary outcomes: mental development

The primary outcomes were the cognitive, expressive language, receptive language and fine motor scores on the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development( Reference Bayley 22 ), which are most commonly used internationally to assess skills acquired in the first 3 years of life. Bayley Scales have been used in Ethiopia( Reference Aboud and Alemu 23 Reference Servili, Medhin and Hanlon 25 ) and elsewhere( Reference Aboud, Singla and Nahil 26 , Reference Yousafzai, Rasheed and Rizvi 27 ). The tests were modified, as is usual, by having an illustrator provide context-relevant pictures for some of the cognitive and language items; administration instructions were followed with the exception that precise timing of certain items with a stopwatch was not done( Reference Servili, Medhin and Hanlon 25 ). Otherwise, almost all of the test objects were appropriate for this setting. Field testing demonstrated high concurrent validity with age (r>0·80) of the four subtests, and the expected urban–rural difference, using children aged between 6 and 24 months. Age-standardised scores from the test manual were used to create comparable scores across the different subscales (theoretic range of 0–19) and a summed composite score out of 76.

Psychosocial stimulation and maternal depressive symptoms

Two variables that might influence mental development and alter as a result of introducing iodised salt are psychosocial stimulation and mothers’ depression( Reference Milgrom and Holt 28 , Reference Aboud and Yousafzai 29 ).

Psychosocial stimulation was measured with a modified version of the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) Inventory, a forty-five-item observation and interview measure of opportunities for stimulation of the child around the home( Reference Aboud and Yousafzai 29 , Reference Bradley and Corwyn 30 ). Modifications were made to individual items to make them suitable for this rural context( Reference Aboud 31 ). Each item received a score for being not observed/not present (=0) or observed/present (=1) for a specific kind of stimulation (e.g. child has any play material; the mother responds verbally to child's talk or gesture). The HOME has been used in many different countries and is strongly associated with mental development( Reference Aboud and Yousafzai 29 ). It could also be affected by iodised salt if mothers themselves in response to higher levels of iodine became more responsive and energetic when interacting with their child as iodine has been associated with higher levels of productivity( Reference Hetzel 32 ). Concurrent validity was good in relation to mental development and mother’s education (r 0·13 to 0·33 and r 0·16, respectively, controlling for age, P=0·0001).

Maternal depressive symptoms were assessed with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale, a twenty-item depressive symptoms (feelings of low self-esteem, sad mood, low energy and worry) frequency rating over the past week, recorded as a 0–3 score( Reference Radloff 33 ). Convergent validity with the Self-Reporting Questionnaire( Reference Beusenberg and Orley 34 ) was good with a subsample (n 30; r 0·61; P=0·005). It was considered important to assess maternal depression because of its potential link to child health and development, and because some depressive symptoms such as lack of energy may be reduced with increasing levels of iodine( Reference Haggerty and Prange 35 ).

Anthropometric and goitre assessment

Age was determined from the date of birth obtained from immunisation cards or parental report using the Ethiopian calendar. Weight and length (or height for ≥24 months), respectively, were measured using a mother–child scale (Tanita WB-100; Tanita Corporation of America) with a precision of 100 g and a portable measuring board (Shorr Productions) with a precision of 0·1 cm. All measurements were recorded using standard procedures, and weight and length/height were taken in duplicate and a third time if the discrepancy was above the World Health Organization cut-off( Reference de Onis 36 ). Weight and length/height were translated into standard scores using the WHO age and sex growth standards( 37 , 38 ). Goitre was assessed by thyroid palpation according to WHO guidelines( 20 ).

Urine and blood

Casual urine samples of about 20 ml were collected in iodine-free paediatric urine collection bags at baseline and in urine cups at endline and transferred into iodine-free urine tubes and protected from evaporation. The samples were then stored at −20°C in the field, transferred in dry ice to the Ethiopian Public Health Institute laboratory in Addis Ababa and stored at −20°C until analysed for iodine concentration, using the digestion method of Sandell–Kolthoff reaction( Reference Sandell and Kolthoff 39 , Reference Pino, Fang and Braverman 40 ). Our partner laboratory enrolled in the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Ensuring the Quality of Urinary Iodine Procedures programme and started controls with round 29 before the study and participated successfully in its external validation on subsequent rounds.

Hb concentration for the assessment of anaemia was measured immediately in a drop using a DIFF-SAFE blood dispenser (Alpha Scientific Corporation) from a venous blood sample. The azide-methemoglobin method( Reference Vanzetti 41 ) with a HemoCueTM 201+ portable photometer (HemoCue, Inc.) was used.

Breast-feeding, diet and illness

Breast-feeding and feeding practices were documented as reported by the mother. Dietary diversity over the past 24 h was calculated from a FFQ, using seven food categories, namely grains, legumes, milk, egg, meat-fish, vitamin A-rich foods and other fruit and vegetables( Reference Daelmans, Dewey and Arimond 42 ), without consideration of whether it was a typical day. The diet diversity score ranged from 0 to 7. Mothers were asked to recall symptoms of infant illness over the 14 d before the interview using standard questions from the 2005 Ethiopia Demographic and Health Survey (EDHS)( 19 ). The number of illnesses reported over the past 14 d was derived for analysis.

Household sociodemographics and salt

Household data reported by the mother included parents’ education and occupation, household size and assets, and water and sanitation facilities based on items from the EDHS( 19 ). Assets score was the sum of the following items where each was given 0 if not possessed and 1 if possessed: bed, chair, table, watch, radio/television, phone, lamp, pump, transport and donkey. The water and sanitation score was the sum of the following items: latrine, waste disposal and source of drinking water. Each item was given a score of 1 if it was appropriate and 0 if not appropriate. Mothers also reported the presence of goitre in any family member. Salt samples were collected for iodine qualitative rapid testing (MBI Kits International) and all positive samples at baseline and a subsample at endline were quantitatively tested by titration( 20 ). Adequately iodised salt was defined as salt with iodine level ≥15 ppm after titration.

Training and data collection

The baseline survey was carried out between October 2011 and April 2012, and the endline survey using the same measures on the same children was conducted between March and October 2013. The villages were completed in the same order at both time points. Graduate psychology students from the local university were trained by psychologists to administer the Bayley Scales to children in their homes. Inter-observer reliability was high at baseline and endline at the end of a 14-d training, with inter-tester correlations ranging from 0·68 to 0·97 and differences in scores between the two testings being non-significant.

At baseline and endline, standardisation sessions for anthropometric measurements were organised. The technical error measurement (TEM) and the coefficient of reliability R were good: weight (baseline: TEM=0·1 kg, R 99·7 %; endline: TEM=0·1 kg, R 99·8 %) and length/height (baseline: TEM= 0·5 cm, R 99·1 %; endline: TEM=0·4 cm, R 99·5 %)( Reference Ulijaszek and Kerr 43 Reference Ayele, Aemere and Gebre 45 ).

The team spent 2 to 3 d in each village at each time point. With the list of children from the previously conducted census, the research assistants consulted with the health extension workers and local guides and moved from house to house to enroll and test the children on the Bayley Scales and interview the mothers. They referred the mother with her child to the village centre (generally, health post, school or church) for the anthropometric measurements, thyroid assessment and blood and urine sampling.

Statistical analyses

Sample size estimation was based on a Bayley subtest standardised score of mean=10, sd 1·5, where α=0·05, power=0·80 and effect size d=0·20. This yielded a sample size of 393/group. Assuming a loss-to-follow-up of 10 % and a cluster size of twenty-six children per village, a design effect of 2·0 was required to accommodate intra-cluster correlation of 0·04, yielding a sample size of 850/group.

Our analytic approach was similar to other studies based on the cluster-randomised design( Reference Kramer, Aboud and Mironova 46 ). The data were analysed with SAS 9.3 (SAS Institute, Inc.). Baseline differences between intervention and control groups were assessed using SAS’s PROC MIXED ANOVA for continuous variables and PROC GLIMMIX for categorical variables, to accommodate clusters. The main intention-to-treat analysis examined differences in raw Bayley scores between children in the intervention and control groups using ANCOVA adjusting for clusters and covariates, namely child age at endline, the corresponding baseline subtest score and variables with differences between groups at baseline. Standardised scores are presented only for comparison purposes. Effect sizes, Cohen’s d, were calculated with the adjusted means and the pooled standard deviation. Missing data were not imputed. Interaction terms were tested for possible effect modifiers.

Results

The trial profile is found in Fig. 1. Attrition rates of 17 % were similar in the intervention and control groups. The attrition analysis showed that infants who remained in the trial (n 1563), compared with those lost to follow-up (n 272), had a higher weight-for-age z score (P=0·045) and water and sanitation score (P=0·028), and had more people in their household (P=0·011). Thus, there were differences between those who completed the full trial and those lost to follow-up. No cluster or child was excluded from the analysis. Differences in sample sizes for different analyses are due to missing data.

Fig. 1 Trial profile. Children ‘missed’ were children who were absent during the data collection period in their village. Children ‘not found’ were children who were neither identified nor recognised during the data collection. ‘Migration’ referred to children who were resettled in another village with their mother.

Infants in the intervention and control groups were similar in baseline characteristics in age, maternal education, depressive symptoms and household assets. Despite randomisation, however, there were some differences between groups (see Table 1). The time between baseline and endline assessment was on average 15·8 months and not different between groups.

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of participating infants and families in the intervention and control groups in Amhara, Ethiopia (Mean values and standard deviations)

HOME, Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment.

* Diet diversity score=sum (grains, legumes, milk, egg, meat-fish, vitamin A-rich foods and fruit and vegetables).

Maximum scores on the Bayley Scales are as follows: cognitive (91), expressive language (48), receptive language (49) and fine motor (66).

Assessed with the HOME Inventory.

§ Assessed with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale.

|| Assets score=sum (bed, chair, table, watch, radio-television, phone, lamp, pump, transport and donkey).

Water and sanitation score=sum (latrine, waste disposal and source of drinking water).

** P values based on ANOVA adjusting for clusters.

The results for iodised salt and UIC reveal that intervention children received greater exposure to iodised salt than controls. At baseline, <10 % of households used iodised salt. By July 2012, 70 % of intervention households sampled had iodised salt, whereas a similar level was reached in control households in October 2012 (66 %). The proportion of households using iodised salt increased to 94·4 % in the intervention group and to 89·3 % in the control group at endline, and adequately iodised salt (≥15 ppm) was found in 54·2 % of intervention households and 54·7 % of control households. At baseline, participants in both groups were severely iodine deficient; 68·0 and 77·1 % of infants had UIC below 50 µg/l in the intervention and control groups, respectively. The ANCOVA of endline scores (Table 2), covarying baseline UIC, showed that both groups improved median iodine status but the difference favoured the intervention children (228·0 v. 155·1 µg/l; P=0·001).

Table 2 Comparison of household iodised salt and children’s iodine status in the intervention and control groups at baseline and endline in Amhara, Ethiopia (Percentages and interquartile ranges (IQR))

ppm, parts per million; UIC, urinary iodine concentration.

* Based on titration.

Samples were obtained at baseline on intervention, n 730; control, n 715; at endline on intervention, n 667; control, n 612 children.

The primary outcome analyses found that children in the intervention group scored higher than controls on the cognitive, receptive language and fine motor subtests at endline (Table 3). The effect of the intervention on expressive language was in the same direction. The composite mental development score combining all four subscores was higher in the intervention group compared with the control group (P=0·010). Effect sizes were small.

Table 3 Effects of iodised salt intervention on infants’ iodine status and development, and maternal depressive symptoms, in Amhara, Ethiopia (Mean values and standard deviations; Cohen’s d and 95 % confidence intervals)

HOME, Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment.

* ANCOVA adjusted for cluster, baseline covariates (water and sanitation score, HOME score, weight-for-age z score) along with corresponding baseline Bayley score of the outcome and age at endline.

Maximum raw scores on the Bayley Scales are as follows: cognitive (91), expressive language (48), receptive language (49) and fine motor (66).

Standardised scores are shown for comparison purpose with other studies.

§ Assessed with the HOME Inventory.

|| Assessed with the Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale.

Analyses of other outcomes demonstrated that the iodised salt intervention had no effect on psychosocial stimulation, but it did reduce the number of depressive symptoms reported by mothers, compared with controls (P=0·003) (Table 3). When water and sanitation, diet and illness were examined in interaction terms with the iodine intervention for the composite Bayley score, effect sizes for the intervention increased from d=0·14 to d=0·23 to 0·28 (Table 4). This indicated that there was a four-point developmental quotient (DQ) difference between intervention and control children under certain conditions, namely better water and sanitation, better diet or some illness.

Table 4 Significant interactions between iodised salt intervention and modifier variables on infants’ mental development outcomes (Mean values and standard deviations; Cohen’s d and 95 % confidence intervals)

* Modifiers’ level/cut-off were set close to median to balance cell sizes as much as possible. Only dietary diversity had a conventional cut-off (set at 4 or more), but this would have left cell sizes too discrepant.

All analyses were conducted using covariates current age and the baseline scores of the corresponding variable.

Water and sanitation score=sum (latrine, waste disposal and source of drinking water).

§ Diet diversity score=sum (grains, legumes, milk, egg, meat-fish, vitamin A-rich-foods and fruit and vegetables).

Discussion

This community-based cluster-randomised trial showed that earlier introduction of iodised salt had an effect on children’s mental development scores. Early introduction of iodised salt improved verbal (receptive language) and non-verbal (cognitive and fine motor) scores, as well as the composite mental development score, of the children in the intervention group over those in the control group. Given the randomised design and the consistency of effects, this study provides strong evidence that introducing iodised salt can positively affect young children’s mental development.

In contrast to our results, supplementation of pregnant women in two RCT yielded moderate effect sizes( Reference Pretell, Torres and Zenteno 6 , Reference Thilly 7 ), whereas supplementation of infants yielded no consistent effect( Reference Cao, Jiang and Dou 14 , Reference Trowbridge 15 ). Our effect sizes were d=0·13 for the composite and between 0·11 and 0·14 for three subtests. These differences suggest that supplementation during infancy is effective, but less so than during fetal development, consistent with theoretical arguments based on the timing of brain development and the role of iodine in brain development( Reference Berbel, Auso and Garcia-Velasco 2 , Reference Zoeller and Rovet 3 , Reference Berbel, Guadano-Ferraz and Martinez 47 Reference Bernal 49 ).

Our effect sizes are small but positive and consistent. They are probably lower than the ‘real effect size’ because our comparison group had delayed availability of iodised salt rather than no availability. Yet they are far below the reported 0·90 effect size reported in a 1994 meta-analysis where the authors reached the conclusion that 13·5 DQ points are lost because of iodine deficiency( Reference Bleichrodt and Born 50 ). Limitations to that meta-analysis include the large age range of participants, the wide variety of sometimes non-validated tests and the non-randomised design of the studies( Reference Bougma, Aboud and Harding 5 ). We presented limited evidence that the intervention area received higher-quality iodised salt earlier than the control areas. However, given the expansive research area, market salt iodine levels were not frequently assessed and therefore we do not have as many salt iodine measures as we would have liked during the whole intervention period. On the basis of these limitations, it is more likely that DQ points lost owing to iodine deficiency could be between our current finding of 2·0 DQ points (effect size of 0·13×sd of 15) and 7·2 DQ points from the two RCT where pregnant women were supplemented( Reference Pretell, Torres and Zenteno 6 , Reference Thilly 7 ).

Some recent and important reviews on nutrition and child development( Reference Black, Victora and Walker 51 ) continue to cite the 1994 meta-analysis( Reference Bleichrodt and Born 50 ) as the reference for the cost of, or loss owing to, iodine deficiency. In our 2013 systematic review and meta-analysis on iodine and child development( Reference Bougma, Aboud and Harding 5 ), we reviewed the papers included in the 1994 meta-analysis and indicated the major limitations of that meta-analysis. The large effect size of 0·90 (13·5 DQ points) is an overestimate, based on our, as well as other, meta-analysis that has been published after 1994. It is time for nutrition and development researchers to use the up-to-date reviews and meta-analyses and stop citing the 1994 overestimated effect size. Finally, given that iodine has a more consistent effect on mental development in early life, researchers should focus on the period between preconception and early childhood rather than school-age children.

Other researchers have suggested that iodine deficiency is associated with depression( Reference Haggerty and Prange 35 , Reference Cleare, McGregor and O'Keane 52 ). Although symptoms of depression and hypothyroidism overlap (e.g. fatigue, lack of energy for activities, trouble making decisions), it is not clear whether biochemical mechanisms involving 5-hydroxytryptamine connect the two( Reference Cleare, McGregor and O'Keane 52 ).

Maternal depressive symptoms were reduced in the intervention group compared with the control group. The effect size of 0·16 is small but noteworthy and not studied or reported in other iodine supplementation studies.

The 2015 national micronutrient survey reported that only 15 % of salt was adequately iodised (≥15 ppm)( 53 ), whereas we had at endline in 2013 about 50 % adequately iodised salt. Therefore, the 2015 survey showed that the quality of the iodised salt is still a problem.

The study has several limitations. Both groups had iodised salt by the endline, and thus the distinction between them was minimised at this point. A randomised design such as ours could be ethical only if we did not withhold iodised salt when it became widely available, from control sites. Thus, the difference between the two treatment groups was in duration of exposure. Monitoring of the intervention indicated that the intervention group received longer exposure to iodised salt (8 to 10 months v. 4 to 6 months) and higher-quality iodised salt earlier than control sites. The Amhara Regional Bureau of Health implemented in May–June 2012 its iodine capsule campaign for children aged 6 to 23 months and pregnant and lactating women after the baseline in our study area. Poppy seed oil iodised capsules have been reported to protect for 10 months( Reference Untoro, Schultink and West 54 ). Capsules were given to children in both intervention and control groups 9–14 months before endline. It may have affected the UIC and the mental development, but we assume that both groups were affected equally. The median UIC for both groups might have been slightly higher owing to the supplements (228 µg/l (intervention) v. 155·0 µg/l (control)); the difference is most likely due to iodised salt intake. This is based on additional data from older preschool-aged children who did not receive the iodised oil supplements and whose endline UIC medians were similarly different: 165·8 µg/l (intervention) and 121·1 µg/l (control)( Reference Aboud, Bougma and Lemma 55 ). However, our ability to detect differences may have been reduced by the capsule’s effect. The children’s iodine intake through the family meal and breast milk( Reference Bouhouch, Bouhouch and Cherkaoui 56 ) was not measured, but UIC indicated that it was sufficient at endline, particularly for intervention children.

The strengths of our study were the randomised design, the large sample size and low attrition, and the measurement of, and adjustment for, covariates. This was an intention-to-treat analysis and was adjusted for the cluster design. The mental development test used, the Bayley Scales, is robust and addresses four domains of mental development. Our data provide strong evidence for the importance of the introduction of iodised salt to improve mental development of children. These findings are relevant for policymakers and governments and are generalisable to countries in similar situations to Ethiopia.

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Husein Mohammed, Valerie Friesen, Daisy R Singla, Aregash Samuel, Cherinet Abuye, Dawd Gashu, Biruk A Abegaz, Yayesew C Getnet, Fasil E Mengestie, Agernesh Alem, Abeyot B Malede, Dilnesaw A Zerfu, Adamu T Belay, Meseret W Mengistie, the research assistants, the national office of Nutrition International (NI) in Ethiopia, the Amhara Regional Bureau of Health and the health extension workers. The authors are grateful to all the mothers and children who participated in the study.

NI, an international not-for-profit organisation, conceived the idea for this study and commissioned and funded McGill University to design and implement it. NI did a site visit during the baseline data collection, provided technical assistance in understanding and modifying salt markets, and reviewed this manuscript before publication. As part of separate, ongoing efforts, NI provided technical and financial support for the USI programme. McGill University was fully responsible for the design, conduct, analysis and writing of the study with no interference from NI.

K. B., F. E. A. and G. S. M. designed the study and prepared the measures; K. B., F. E. A., T. M. L. and G. S. M. trained the research assistants; K. B., F. E. A., E. A. F. and G. S. M. conducted the data management and/or analysis; K. B. implemented the fieldwork and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors edited the report and approved the final manuscript.

The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest.

References

1. Black, MM, Walker, SP, Fernald, LC, et al. (2017) Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course. Lancet 389, 7790.Google Scholar
2. Berbel, P, Auso, E, Garcia-Velasco, JV, et al. (2001) Role of thyroid hormones in the maturation and organisation of rat barrel cortex. Neuroscience 107, 383394.Google Scholar
3. Zoeller, RT & Rovet, J (2004) Timing of thyroid hormone action in the developing brain: clinical observations and experimental findings. J Neuroendocrinol 16, 809818.Google Scholar
4. Delange, F (1994) The disorders induced by iodine deficiency. Thyroid 4, 107128.Google Scholar
5. Bougma, K, Aboud, F, Harding, K, et al. (2013) Iodine and mental development of children 5 years old and under: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Nutrients 5, 13841416.Google Scholar
6. Pretell, EA, Torres, T, Zenteno, V, et al. (1972) Prophylaxis of endemic goiter with iodized oil in rural Peru. Adv Exp Med Biol 30, 249265.Google Scholar
7. Thilly, CH (1981) Psychomotor development in regions with endemic goiter. In Fetal Brain Disorders: Recent Approaches to the Problem of Mental Deficiency, pp. 265282 [BS Hetzel and RM Smith, editors]. Amsterdam; New York; New York, NY: Elsevier/North-Holland Biomedical Press; Sole distributors for the USA and Canada, Elsevier North-Holland.Google Scholar
8. Connolly, KJ, Pharoah, PO & Hetzel, BS (1979) Fetal iodine deficiency and motor performance during childhood. Lancet ii, 11491151.Google Scholar
9. Untoro, J (1999) Use of oral iodized oil to control iodine deficiency in Indonesia. PhD Thesis, Wageningen Agricultural University.Google Scholar
10. van den Briel, T, West, CE, Bleichrodt, N, et al. (2000) Improved iodine status is associated with improved mental performance of schoolchildren in Benin. Am J Clin Nutr 72, 11791185.Google Scholar
11. Huda, SN, Grantham-McGregor, SM & Tomkins, A (2001) Cognitive and motor functions of iodine-deficient but euthyroid children in Bangladesh do not benefit from iodized poppy seed oil (Lipiodol). J Nutr 131, 7277.Google Scholar
12. Zimmermann, MB, Connolly, K, Bozo, M, et al. (2006) Iodine supplementation improves cognition in iodine-deficient schoolchildren in Albania: a randomized, controlled, double-blind study. Am J Clin Nutr 83, 108114.Google Scholar
13. Gordon, RC, Rose, MC, Skeaff, SA, et al. (2009) Iodine supplementation improves cognition in mildly iodine-deficient children. Am J Clin Nutr 90, 12641271.Google Scholar
14. Cao, XY, Jiang, XM, Dou, ZH, et al. (1994) Timing of vulnerability of the brain to iodine deficiency in endemic cretinism. N Engl J Med 331, 17391744.Google Scholar
15. Trowbridge, FL (1972) Intellectual assessment in primitive societies, with a preliminary report of a study of the effects of early iodine supplementation on intelligence. Adv Exp Med Biol 30, 137149.Google Scholar
16. Melse-Boonstra, A & Jaiswal, N (2010) Iodine deficiency in pregnancy, infancy and childhood and its consequences for brain development. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab 24, 2938.Google Scholar
17. Andersson, M, Karumbunathan, V & Zimmermann, MB (2012) Global iodine status in 2011 and trends over the past decade. J Nutr 142, 744750.Google Scholar
18. Abuye, C, Berhane, Y, Akalu, G, et al. (2007) Prevalence of goiter in children 6 to 12 years of age in Ethiopia. Food Nutr Bull 28, 391398.Google Scholar
19. Central Statistical Agency (Ethiopia), ORC Macro (2006) Ethiopia Demographic and Health Survey 2005. Addis Ababa and Calverton, MD: Central Statistical Agency and ORC Macro.Google Scholar
20. World Health Organization (2007) Assessment of Iodine Deficiency Disorders and Monitoring their Elimination. A Guide for Program Managers. Geneva: WHO/UNICEF/ICCIDD.Google Scholar
21. Campbell, MK, Piaggio, G, Elbourne, DR, et al. (2012) Consort 2010 statement: extension to cluster randomised trials. BMJ 345, e5661.Google Scholar
22. Bayley, N (2006) Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
23. Aboud, FE & Alemu, T (1995) Nutrition, maternal responsiveness and mental development of Ethiopian children. Soc Sci Med 41, 725732.Google Scholar
24. Drewett, R, Wolke, D, Asefa, M, et al. (2001) Malnutrition and mental development: is there a sensitive period? A nested case-control study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 42, 181187.Google Scholar
25. Servili, C, Medhin, G, Hanlon, C, et al. (2010) Maternal common mental disorders and infant development in Ethiopia: the P-MaMiE Birth Cohort. BMC Public Health 10, 693.Google Scholar
26. Aboud, FE, Singla, DR, Nahil, MI, et al. (2013) Effectiveness of a parenting program in Bangladesh to address early childhood health, growth and development. Soc Sci Med 97, 250258.Google Scholar
27. Yousafzai, AK, Rasheed, MA, Rizvi, A, et al. (2014) Effect of integrated responsive stimulation and nutrition interventions in the Lady Health Worker programme in Pakistan on child development, growth, and health outcomes: a cluster-randomised factorial effectiveness trial. Lancet 384, 12821293.Google Scholar
28. Milgrom, J & Holt, C (2014) Early intervention to protect the mother-infant relationship following postnatal depression: study protocol for a randomised controlled trial. Trials 15, 385.Google Scholar
29. Aboud, FE & Yousafzai, AK (2015) Global health and development in early childhood. Annu Rev Psychol 66, 433457.Google Scholar
30. Bradley, RH & Corwyn, RF (2005) Caring for children around the world: a view from HOME. Int J Behav Dev 29, 468478.Google Scholar
31. Aboud, FE (2007) Evaluation of an early childhood parenting programme in rural Bangladesh. J Health Popul Nutr 25, 313.Google ScholarPubMed
32. Hetzel, BS (1988) The Prevention and Control of Iodine Deficiency Disorders. Discussion by F Delange, JB Stanbury and FE Viteri. Nutrition Policy Discussion Paper no. 3. Geneva: United Nations.Google Scholar
33. Radloff, LS (1977) The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1, 385401.Google Scholar
34. Beusenberg, M & Orley, J (1994) A User's Guide to Self Reporting Questionnaire (SRQ). Geneva: Division of Mental Health, WHO.Google Scholar
35. Haggerty, JJ Jr & Prange, AJ Jr (1995) Borderline hypothyroidism and depression. Annu Rev Med 46, 3746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36. World Health Organization Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group, de Onis, M (2006) Reliability of anthropometric measurements in the WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study. Acta Paediatr Suppl 450, 3846.Google Scholar
37. World Health Organization Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group (2006) WHO Child Growth Standards: Length/Height-for-Age, Weight-for-age, Weight-for-Length, Weight-for-Height and Body Mass Index-for-Age: Methods and Development. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
38. World Health Organization Multicentre Growth Reference Study Group (2007) WHO Child Growth Standards: Head Circumference-for-Age, Arm Circumference-for-Age, Triceps Skinfold-for-Age and Subscapular Skinfold-for-Age: Methods and Development. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
39. Sandell, EB & Kolthoff, IM (1937) Micro determination of iodine by a catalytic method. Microchimica Acta 1, 925.Google Scholar
40. Pino, S, Fang, SL & Braverman, LE (1996) Ammonium persulfate: a safe alternative oxidizing reagent for measuring urinary iodine. Clin Chem 42, 239243.Google Scholar
41. Vanzetti, G (1966) An azide-methemoglobin method for hemoglobin determination in blood. J Lab Clin Med 67, 116126.Google Scholar
42. Daelmans, B, Dewey, K & Arimond, M (2009) New and updated indicators for assessing infant and young child feeding. Food Nutr Bull 30, S256S262.Google Scholar
43. Ulijaszek, SJ & Kerr, DA (1999) Anthropometric measurement error and the assessment of nutritional status. Br J Nutr 82, 165177.Google Scholar
44. World Health Organization (2006) Reliability of anthropometric measurements in the WHO Multicentre Growth Reference Study. Acta Paediatr Suppl 450, 3846.Google Scholar
45. Ayele, B, Aemere, A, Gebre, T, et al. (2012) Reliability of measurements performed by community-drawn anthropometrists from rural Ethiopia. PLOS ONE 7, e30345.Google Scholar
46. Kramer, MS, Aboud, F, Mironova, E, et al. (2008) Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: new evidence from a large randomized trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry 65, 578584.Google Scholar
47. Berbel, P, Guadano-Ferraz, A, Martinez, M, et al. (1993) Organization of auditory callosal connections in hypothyroid adult rats. Eur J Neurosci 5, 14651478.Google Scholar
48. Berbel, P, Guadano-Ferraz, A, Angulo, A, et al. (1994) Role of thyroid hormones in the maturation of interhemispheric connections in rats. Behav Brain Res 64, 914.Google Scholar
49. Bernal, J (2005) Thyroid hormones and brain development. Vitam Horm 71, 95122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
50. Bleichrodt, N & Born, PM (1994) A meta-analysis of research on iodine and its relationship to cognitive development. In The Damaged Brain of Iodine Deficiency: Cognitive, Behavioral, Neuromotor and Educative Aspects, pp. 195200 [JB Stanbury, editor]. New York: Cognizant Communication Corporation.Google Scholar
51. Black, RE, Victora, CG, Walker, SP, et al. (2013) Maternal and child undernutrition and overweight in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet 382, 427451.Google Scholar
52. Cleare, AJ, McGregor, A & O'Keane, V (1995) Neuroendocrine evidence for an association between hypothyroidism, reduced central 5-HT activity and depression. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 43, 713719.Google Scholar
53. Ethiopian Public Health Institute (2016) Ethiopian National Micronutrient Survey. Addis Ababa: Ethiopian Public Health Institute.Google Scholar
54. Untoro, J, Schultink, W, West, CE, et al. (2006) Efficacy of oral iodized peanut oil is greater than that of iodized poppy seed oil among Indonesian schoolchildren. Am J Clin Nutr 84, 12081214.Google Scholar
55. Aboud, FE, Bougma, K, Lemma, T, et al. (2017) Evaluation of the effects of iodized salt on the mental development of preschool-aged children: a cluster randomized trial in northern Ethiopia. Matern Child Nutr 13, e12322.Google Scholar
56. Bouhouch, RR, Bouhouch, S, Cherkaoui, M, et al. (2014) Direct iodine supplementation of infants versus supplementation of their breastfeeding mothers: a double-blind, randomised, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet Diabetes Endocrinol 2, 197209.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Trial profile. Children ‘missed’ were children who were absent during the data collection period in their village. Children ‘not found’ were children who were neither identified nor recognised during the data collection. ‘Migration’ referred to children who were resettled in another village with their mother.

Figure 1

Table 1 Baseline characteristics of participating infants and families in the intervention and control groups in Amhara, Ethiopia (Mean values and standard deviations)

Figure 2

Table 2 Comparison of household iodised salt and children’s iodine status in the intervention and control groups at baseline and endline in Amhara, Ethiopia (Percentages and interquartile ranges (IQR))

Figure 3

Table 3 Effects of iodised salt intervention on infants’ iodine status and development, and maternal depressive symptoms, in Amhara, Ethiopia (Mean values and standard deviations; Cohen’s d and 95 % confidence intervals)

Figure 4

Table 4 Significant interactions between iodised salt intervention and modifier variables on infants’ mental development outcomes (Mean values and standard deviations; Cohen’s d and 95 % confidence intervals)