We use cookies to distinguish you from other users and to provide you with a better experience on our websites. Close this message to accept cookies or find out how to manage your cookie settings.
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)
References
Allee, W. C. (1938) Cooperation among animals. Abelard-Schuman. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Allport, F. H. (1955) Theories of perception and the concept of structure. Wiley. [GDW]Google Scholar
Ardrey, R. (1961) African genesis. Atheneum. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Aslin, R. N. (1981) Experiential influences and sensitive periods in development: A unified model. In: Development of perception: Psychobiological perspectives, vol. 2, ed. Aslin, R. N., Alberts, J. R. & Petersen, M. R.. Academic Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Aslin, R. N. & Pisoni, D. B. (1980) Some developmental processes in speech perception. In: Child phonology: Perception and production, ed. Yeni-Komshian, G. H., Kavanagh, J. F. & Ferguson, C. A.. Academic Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Azrin, N. H., Hutchinson, R. R. & Laughlin, R. (1965) The opportunity for aggression as an operant reinforcer during aversive stimulation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior8:171–80. [taBFS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barash, D. P. & Lipton, J. E. (in press) Overcoming the Neanderthal mentality. McGraw-Hill. [DPB]Google Scholar
Barkow, J. (1976) Attention structure and the evolution of human psychological characteristics. In: The social structure of attention, ed. Chance, M. R. A. & Larsen, R. R.. Wiley. [JHB]Google Scholar
Barkow, J. (1983) Begged questions in behavior and evolution. In: Animal models of human behavior, ed. Davey, G.. [JHB]Google Scholar
Beach, F. A. (1950) The snark was a boojum. American Psychologist5:115–24. [JPH, taBFS]Google Scholar
Beach, F. A. (1955) The descent of instinct. Psychological Review62:401–10. [GMB]Google Scholar
de Beer, G. (1940) Embryos and ancestors. Oxford University Press. 3d rev. ed.1958. [GG]Google Scholar
Blanchard, R. J. & Blanchard, D. C. (1980) The organization of aggressive behaviors in rodents. In: The biology of aggression, ed. Brain, P. F. & Benton, D.. Nordhoof. [DCB]Google Scholar
Blanshard, B. (1967) The problem of consciousness – a debate. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research27:317–24. [AMC, rBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonner, J. T., ed. (1982) Evolution and development. Springer Verlag [SNS]Google Scholar
Branden, N. (1963) The contradiction of determinism. Objectivist Newsletter2:17–20. [AMC]Google Scholar
Breland, K. & Breland, M. (1961) The misbehavior of organisms. American Psychologist16:681–84. [GMB, taBFS]Google Scholar
Brown, P. L. & Jenkins, H. M. (1968) Auto-shaping of the pigeon's key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior11:1–8. [AMC, rBFS]Google Scholar
Brunjes, P. C. & Alberts, J. R. (1979) Olfactory stimulation induces filial preferences for huddling in rat pups. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology93:548–55. [GG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bull, J. J. & Vogt, R. C. (1979) Temperature-dependent sex determination in turtles. Science206:1186–88. [DW]Google Scholar
Bullock, T. H. (1970) The reliability of neurones. Jacques Loeb memorial lectures, Woods Hole, Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution and Marine Biological Laboratory Massachusetts. [GDW]Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M. (1973) Instinct and innate behavior: Toward an ethological psychology. In: The study of behavior, ed. Nevin, J. A. & Reynolds, G. S.. Scott, Foresman & Co. [GMB]Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M. (1977) Ontogeny of communication. In: How animals communicate, ed. Sebeok, T. A.. Indiana University Press. [GMB]Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960) Blind variation and selective retention in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review67:380–400. [JERS]Google Scholar
Caraco, T. (1980) On foraging time allocation in a stochastic environment. Ecology61:119–28. [AK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caraco, T. (1983) White-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia Peucophrys): Foraging preferences in a risky environment. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology12:63–69. [AK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caraco, T., Martindale, S. & Whittam, T. S. (1980) An empirical demonstration of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Animal Behaviour28:820–30. [AK]Google Scholar
Catania, A. C. (1981) Contingency, continguity, correlation, and the concept of causation. In: Advances in the study of behaviour, vol. 2, Predictability, correlation and contiguity. Wiley. [JERS]Google Scholar
Clark, M. M. & Galef, B. G.Jr (1979) A sensitive period for the maintenance of emotionality in Mongolian gerbils. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology93:200–210. [GG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clowes, J. S. & Wassermann, G. D. (1984) Genetic control theory of developmental events. Bulletin of Mathematical Biology, in press. [GDW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craik, K. J. W. (1943). The nature of explanation. Cambridge University Press. [SKR]Google Scholar
Crawford, L. (1983) Local contrast and memory windows as proximate foraging mechanisms. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie63:283–93. [GMB]Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859) On the origin of species by means of natural selection. R. West. [JPH]Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1868) The variation of animals and plants under domestication. John Murray. [MTG]Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872) The expression of the emotions in man and animals. R. West. [taBFS]Google Scholar
De Laguna, G. (1927) Speech: Its function and development. Yale University Press. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Delius, J. D. (1970) The ontogeny of behaviour. In: The neurosciences second study program, ed. Schmitt, F. O.. Rockefeller University Press. [JDD]Google Scholar
Delius, J. D. (1985) Behaviour, free for all. In: Behavior analysis and contemporary psychology, ed. Lowe, C. F., Richelle, M., Blackman, D. E. & Bradshaw, C. M.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. In press. [JDD]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1983) Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The “Panglossian paradigm” defended. Behavioral and Brain Sciences6:343–55. [MTG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denny, M. R., ed. (1980) Comparative psychology. Ẇiley, [GMB]Google Scholar
Diderot, D. (1796) Supplément au Voyage de Bougainville. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Drachman, D. B. & Sokoloff, L. (1966) The role of movement in embryonic joint development. Developmental Biology14:401–20. [DW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dubos, R. (1965) Humanistic biology. American Scientist53:4–19. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1975) Ethology: The biology of behavior. 2d ed.Holt, Rinehart & Winston. [GMB]Google Scholar
Dubos, R. (1979) Human ethology:Concepts and implications for the sciences of man. Behavioral and Brain Sciences2:1–57. [IE-E, DW]Google Scholar
Einstein, A. (1960) Fund-raising telegram for the Emergency Committee of Atomic Scientists, 23 May 1946. In: Einstein on peace, ed. Nathan, O. & Norden, H.. Simon & Schuster. [DPB]Google Scholar
Eldredge, N. & Gould, S. J. (1972) Punctuated equilibria: An alternative to phyletic gradualism. In: Models in paleobiology, ed. Schopf, T. J. M.. Freeman, Cooper & Co. [AK]Google Scholar
Erlenmeyer-Kimling, E., Hirsch, J. & Weiss, J. M. (1962) Studies in experimental behavior genetics: III. Selection and hybridization analyses of individual differences in the sign of geotaxis. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology55:722–31. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1967) The biological basis of personality. C. C. Thomas. [HJE]Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1979) The conditioning model of neurosis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences2:155–99. [HJE]Google Scholar
Eysenck, H. J. (1982) Neobehavioristic (S-R) theory. In: Contemporary behavior therapy, ed. Wilson, G. T. & Franks, C. M.. Guilford Press. [HJE]Google Scholar
Fentress, J. C. (1973) Development of grooming in mice with amputated forelimbs. Science179:704–5. [IE-E]Google Scholar
Ferguson, M. W. J. & Joanen, T. (1982) Temperature of egg incubation determines sex in Alligator mississippiensis. Nature296:850–53. [DW]Google Scholar
Ferster, C. B. & Skinner, B. F. (1957) Schedules of reinforcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Fishbein, H. D. (1976) Evolution, development, and children's learning. Goodyear Publishing Company. [AJP]Google Scholar
Fisher, R. A. (1918) The correlation between relatives on the supposition of Mendelian inheritance. Transactions of the Royal Society (Edinburgh) 52:399–433. [SAA]Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1939) Moses and monotheism. Random House. [AK]Google Scholar
Friedmann, H. (1956) Quoted in article entitled “African honey-guides.”Science123:55. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Garcia, J., Clarke, J. & Hankins, W. G. (1973) Natural responses to scheduled rewards. In: Perspectives in ethology, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. & Klopfer, P.. Plenum Press. [JERS]Google Scholar
Garcia, J. & Koelling, R. A. (1966) The relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science5:121–22. [JDD]Google Scholar
Garstang, W. (1922) The theory of recapitulation: A critical restatement of the biogenetic law. Journal of the Linnean Society of London35:81–101. [GG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghiselin, M. T. (1969a) The evolution of hermaphroditism among animals. Quarterly Review of Biology44:189–208. [MTG]Google Scholar
Ghiselin, M. T. (1969b) The triumph of the Darwinian method. University of California Press. [MTG]Google Scholar
Ghiselin, M. T. (1974) The economy of nature and the evolution of sex. University of California Press. [MTG]Google Scholar
Ghiselin, M. T. (1981) Categories, life, and thinking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences4:269–83. [MTG]Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1971) Development of species identification in birds. University of Chicago Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1976a) Conceptions of prenatal development: Behavioral embryology. Psychological Review83:215–34. [GG]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, G. (1976b) The roles of experience in the development of behavior and the nervous system. In: Neural and behavioral specificity, ed. Gottlieb, G.. Academic Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1981) Roles of early experience in species-specific perceptual development. In: Development of perception: Psychobiological perspectives, vol. 1, ed. Aslin, R. N., Alberts, J. R. & Petersen, M. R.. Academic Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1977) Ontogeny and phylogeny. Harvard University Press. [MTG, GG, AK]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Lewontin, R. (1979) The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of LondonB205:581–98. [MTG, AK]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. & Vrba, E. S. (1982) Exaptation – a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology8:4–15. [AJP, rBFS]Google Scholar
Gray, D. B., ed. (1985) Developmental behavioral genetics and learning. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [RP]Google Scholar
Gray, P. H. (1963) The descriptive study of imprinting in birds from 1863 to 1953. Journal of General Psychology68:333–46. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Gregory, R. L. (1969) On how so little information controls so much behavior. In: Towards a theoretical biology, two sketches, ed. Waddington, C. H.. Edinburgh University Press. [SK]Google Scholar
Haeckel, E. (1891) Anthropogenie oder entwicklungsgeschichte des Menschen: Keimes- und Stammes-Geschichte. 4th rev. enl. ed.Engelmann. [GG]Google Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1969) How an instinct is learned. Scientific American221:98–106. [DW]Google Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1982a) Evolution and behavior: An iconoclastic view. In: Learning, development, and culture, ed. Plotkin, H. C.. Wiley. [JPH]Google Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1982b) Ontogeny: Toward a general theoretical framework for ethology. In: Perspectives in ethology, vol. 5, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. & Klopfer, P. H.. Plenum Press. [JPH]Google Scholar
Hailman, J. P. (1984) Historical notes on the biology of learning. In: Issues in the ecological study of learning, ed. Johnston, T. D. & Pietrewicz, A. T.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [JPH]Google Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology7:1–52. [JLB]Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949) The organization of behavior. Wiley. [GDW]Google Scholar
Heinroth, O. (1911) Beiträge zur Biologie, namentlich Ethologie und Psychologie der Anatiden. Verhandlung V. Internationale Ornithologische Kongress. [JPH]Google Scholar
Heller, R. & Milinsky, M. (1979) Optimal foraging of sticklebacks on swarming prey. Animal Behaviour27:1127–41. [AK]Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. (1966) Animal behaviour: A Synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology. 1st ed.McGraw-Hill. [JDD]Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1973) Constraints on learning: Limitations and predispositions. Academic Press. [AK]Google Scholar
Hirsch, J. (1963) Behavior genetics and individuality understood. Science142:1436–42. [taBFS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirsh, H. V. B. & Jacobson, M. (1975) The perfectible brain: Principles of neuronal development. In: Handbook of psychobiology, ed. Gazzaniga, M. S. & Blakemore, C.. Academic Press. [HCP]Google Scholar
Ho, M. W. & Saunders, P. T. (1979) Beyond neoDarwinism: An epigenetic approach to evolution. Journal of Theoretical Biology78:673–91. [HCP]Google Scholar
Ho, M. W. & Saunders, P. T. (1982) The epigenetic approach to the evolution of organisms. In: Learning, development and culture: Essays in evolutionary epistemology, ed. Plotkin, H. C.. Wiley. [HCP]Google Scholar
Hoffman, H. S., Schiff, D., Adams, J. & Serle, J. L. (1966) Enhanced distress vocalization through selective reinforcement. Science151:352–54. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Hogan, J. A. & Roper, T. J. (1978) A comparison of the properties of different reinforcers. Advances in the Study of Behavior8:155–255. [JAH]Google Scholar
Holland, P. C. (1977) Conditioned stimulus as a determinant of the form of the Pavlovian conditioned response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes3:77–104. [JERS]Google ScholarPubMed
Holliday, R. & Maynard Smith, J., ed. (1979) The evolution of adaptation by natural selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London205: whole issue. [JAH]Google Scholar
Hollis, K. L. (1982) Pavlovian conditioning of signal-centered action patterns and autonomic behavior: A biological analysis of function. Advances in the Study of Behavior12:1–64. [JERS]Google Scholar
Houston, A. I. & McNamara, J. M. (1982) A sequential approach to risk taking. Animal Behaviour30:1260–61. [AK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huxley, J. (1964) Psychometabolism: General and Lorenzian. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine7:399–432. [tarBFS]Google Scholar
Jacobson, M. (1969) Development of specific neuronal connections. Science163:543–47. [GDW]Google Scholar
James, W. (1890) The principles of psychology. Henry Holt & Co. [JPH]Google Scholar
Johnston, T. D. (1982) Learning and the evolution of developmental systems. in: Learning, development, and culture: Essays in evolutionary epistemology, ed. Plotkin, H. C.. Wiley. [GG]Google Scholar
Johnston, T. D. & Gottlieb, G. (1981) Epigenesis and phylogenesis: Reordering the priorities. Behavioral and Brain Sciences4:243. [HCP]Google Scholar
Kacelnik, A., Houston, A., & Krebs, J. R. (1981) Optimal foraging and territorial defence in the great tit (Parus major). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology8:35–40. [AK]Google Scholar
Kaplan, S. (1977) Participation in the design process: A cognitive approach. In: Perspectives on environment and behavior, ed. Stokols, D.. Plenum Press. [SK]Google Scholar
Kaplan, S. (1983) A model of person-environment compatibility. Environment and Behavior15:311–32. [SK]Google Scholar
Kaplan, S. & Kaplan, R. (1982) Cognition and environment: Functioning in an uncertain world. Praeger. [SK]Google Scholar
Kavanau, J. L. (1964) Behavior: Confinement, adaptation, and compulsory regimes in laboratory studies. Science143:490. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Kelleher, R. T. (1962) Variables and behavior. American Psychologist17:659–60. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Kerr, L. M., Ostapoff, E. M. & Rubel, E. W. (1979) Influence of acoustic experience on the ontogeny of frequency generalization gradients on the chicken. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes5:97–115. [GG]Google Scholar
Kettlewell, H. B. D. (1961) The phenomenon of industrial melanism in Lepidoptera. Annual Review of Entomology6:245–62. [SAA]Google Scholar
Kortlandt, A. (1965) On the essential morphological basis for human culture. Current Anthropology6:320–25. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Krischke, N. (1983) Beitrage zur Ontogenese der Flug- und Manövrierfähigkeit der Haustaube (Columba livia var. domestica), Behaviour84:265–86. [GG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1921) Giving up instincts in psychology. Journal of Philosophy17:645–64. [GMB]Google Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1924) A psychology wíthout heredity. Psychological Review31:427–51. [GMB]Google Scholar
Kuo, Z. Y. (1967) The dynamics of behavior development. Random House. (IE-E]Google Scholar
Lashley, K. (1938) Experimental analysis of instinctive behavior. Psychological Review45:445–71. [GMB]Google Scholar
Lee, S. M. & Bressler, R. (1981) Prevention of diabetic nephropathy by diet control in the db/db mouse. Diabetes30:106–11. [DW]Google Scholar
Lehrman, D. S. (1953) A critique of Konrad Lorenz's theory of instinctive behavior. Quarterly Review of Biology28:337–63. [GPB, IE-E, JPH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehrman, D. S. (1970) Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature-nurture problem. In: Development and evolution of behavior, ed. Aronson, L. R., Tobach, E., Lehrman, D. S. & Rosenblatt, J. S.. Freeman. [GG, JAH]Google Scholar
Leibniz, G. W. (1695) New system of the nature and communication of substances, as well as of the union existing between the soul and the body. Transl. in Parkinson, G. H. R. (1973) Leibniz: Philosophical writings. J. M. Dent & Sons. [MTG]Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (1982) Organism and environment. In: Learning, development, and culture, ed. Plotkin, H. C.. Wiley. [SNS]Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (1983) Elementary errors about evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences6:367–68. [MTG]Google Scholar
Locke, E. A. (1966) The contradiction of epiphenomenalism. British Journal of Psychology57:203–4. [AMC]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenz, K. (1961) Phylogenetische Anpassung und adaptive Modifikation des Verhaltens. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie18:139–87. [GPB]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1965) Evolution and modification of behavior. University of Chicago Press. [GPB, JDD, IE-E, GG, JPH, taBFS]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1966) On aggression. Harcourt, Brace & World. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1981) The foundations of ethology. Simon and Schuster. [DW]Google Scholar
McFarland, D. J. & Houston, A. I. (1981) Quantitative ethology: The state space approach. Pitman. [AK]Google Scholar
MacKay, D. M. (1951) Mindlike behaviour in artifacts. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science2:105–21. [GDW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacKay, D. M. (1952) Mentality in machines. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society Suppl. 26:61–86. [GDW]Google Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1974) The psychology of animal learning. Academic Press. [AK]Google Scholar
McNamara, J. M. & Houston, A. I. (1982) Short-term behaviour and life-time fitness. In: Functional ontogeny, ed. McFarland, D. J.. Pitman. [AK]Google Scholar
Marler, P. & Hamilton, W. J. (1966) Mechanisms of animal behavior. Wiley. [JDD]Google ScholarPubMed
Mayr, E. (1963) Animal species and evolution. Harvard University Press. [JERS]Google Scholar
Metz, J. A. J. (1971) State space models for animal behaviour. Annals of Systems Research6:65–109. [AK]Google Scholar
Milinski, M. & Heller, R. (1978) Influence of a predator on the optimal foraging behaviour of sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus L.)Nature275:642–44. [AK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1969) Psychology as a means of promoting human welfare. American Psychologist24:1063–75. [SK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, J. A. (1978a) A summary and criticism of Skinner's early theory of learning. Canadian Psychological Review19:215–23. [AMC]Google Scholar
Mills, J. A. (1978b) A summary and criticism of Skinner's second theory of learning. Canadian Psychological Review19:328–37. [AMC]Google Scholar
Mineka, S. & Suomi, S. J. (1978) Social separation in monkeys. Psychological Bulletin85:1376–1400. [HCP]Google Scholar
Moore, B. R. (1973) The role of directed Pavlovian reactions in simple instrumental learning in the pigeon. In: Constraints on learning, ed. Hinde, R. A. & Stevenson-Hinde, J.. Academic Press. [JERS]Google Scholar
Mortenson, F. J. (1975) Animal behavior: Theory and research. Brooks/Cole. [GMB]Google Scholar
Nanney, D. L. (1977) Molecules and morphologies: The perpetuation of pattern in the ciliated protozoa. Journal of Protozoology24:27–35. [DW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nevin, J. A. & Reynolds, G. S., eds. (1973) The study of behavior: Learning, motivation, emotion, and instinct. Scott, Foresman & Co. [GMB]Google Scholar
Newman, O. (1980) Community of interest. Doubleday. [SK]Google Scholar
Oppenheimer, J. M. (1967) Essays in the history of embryology and biology. MIT Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Osgood, C. (1962) An alternative to war or surrender. University of Illinois Press. [DPB]Google Scholar
Palay, S. L. (1967) Principles of cellular organization. In: The neuwsciences, ed. Quarton, G. C., Melnechuk, T. & Schmitt, F. O.. Rockefeller University Press. [GDW]Google Scholar
Patten, B. C. (1982) Environs: Relativistic elementary particles for ecology. American Naturalist119:179–219. [SNS]Google Scholar
Perkel, D. H. (1970) Spike trains as carriers of information. In: The neurosciences second study program, ed. Schmitt, F. O.. Rockefeller University Press. [GDW]Google Scholar
Peterson, N. (1960) Control of behavior by presentation of an imprinted stimulus. Science132:1395–96. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1979) Behaviour and evolution. Routledge and Kegan Paul. [HCP]Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Garcia, R. (1982) Psicogenesis e historia de la ciencia. Siglo XXI. [AK]Google Scholar
Plomin, R. (1983) Developmental behavioral genetics. Child Development54:253–59. [RP]Google Scholar
Plomin, R. (1985) Behavioral genetics, behavioral analysis, and development. In: Developmental behavioral genetics and learning, ed. Gray, D. B.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [RP]Google Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C. & McClearn, G. E. (1980) Behavioral genetics. Freeman. [RP]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. & Odling-Smee, F. J. (1979) Learning, change and evolution. Advances in the Study of Behaviour10:1–41. [HCP]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. & Odling-Smee, F. J. (1981) A multiple level model of evolution and its implications for sociobiology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences4:225–68. [HCP]Google Scholar
Popper, K. R. (1957) The open society and its enemies. Routledge & Kegan Paul. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Pringle, J. W. S. (1951) On the parallel between learning and evolution. Behaviour3:174–215. [JDD]Google Scholar
Raff, R. A. & Kaufman, T. C. (1983) Embryos, genes, and evolution. Macmillan. [JERS]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. (1967) Pavlovian conditioning and its proper control procedures. Psychological Review74:71–80. [JERS]Google Scholar
Resnick, L. B. (1983) Mathematics and science learning: A new conception. Science220:477–78. [SK]Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1884) Mental evolution in animals. Keegan, Paul, Trench & Co. [JPH]Google Scholar
Romanes, G. J. (1889) Mental evolution in man. D. Appleton & Co. [JPH]Google Scholar
Schaller, G. B. (1964) The year of the gorilla. University of Chicago Press. [SK]Google Scholar
Sebeok, T. A. (1965) Animal communication. Science147:1006–14. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Segall, M. H. (1979) Cross-cultural psychology. Brooks/Cole. [IE-E]Google Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970) On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review77:406–18. [JDD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (1975) Reinforcement and the organization of behavior in golden hamsters: Hunger, environment and food reinforcement. Journal of Experimental Psychology104:56–87. [JERS]Google Scholar
Siegler, R. S. (1983) Review of The development of mathematical thinking. Science221:1042–43. [SK]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1932a) Drive and reflex strength, I. Journal of General Psychology6:22–37. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1932b) Drive and reflex strength, II. Journal of General Psychology6:38–48. [rBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1937) Two types of conditioned reflex: A reply to Konorski and Miller. Journal of General Psychology16:272–79. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938) The behavior of organisms. Appleton-Century. [GMB, AMC, JDD, tarBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1948a) “Superstition” in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology38:168–72. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1948b) Walden two. Macmillan. [rBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1950) Are theories of learning necessary?Psychological Review57:193–216. [AMC]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953) Science and human behavior. Macmillan. [AMC, rBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1957) Verbal behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1960) Pigeons in a pelican. American Psychologist15:28–37. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1969) Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [GMB]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1971) Beyond freedom and dignity. Knopf. [rBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1979) The shaping of a behaviorist. Alfred A. Knopf. [RP]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1981) Selection by consequences. Science213:501–4. [SNS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1982) Contrived reinforcement. Behavior Analyst5:3–8. [rBFS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1983a) Can the experimental analysis of behavior rescue psychology?Behavior Analyst6:9–17. [rBFS, JERS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1983b) Origins of a behaviorist. Psychology Today17(9):22–33. [AMC]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. & Morse, W. H. (1957) Concurrent activity under fixed-interval reinforcement. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology50:279–81. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Sommerhoff, G. (1950) Analytical biology. Oxford University Press. [GDW]Google Scholar
Sommerhoff, G. (1974) Logic of the living brain. Wiley. [GDW]Google Scholar
Sperry, R. W. (1971) How a developing brain gets itself properly wired for adaptive function. In: The biopsychology of development, ed. Tobach, E., Aronson, L. R. & Shaw, E.. Academic Press. [IE-E]Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. (1977) Schedule-induced behavior. In: Handbook of operant behavior, ed. Honig, W. K., & Staddon, J. E. R.. Prentice-Hall. [JAH]Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. (1983) Adaptive behavior and learning. Cambridge University Press. [JERS]Google Scholar
Staddon, J. E. R. & Simmelhag, V. (1971) The “superstition” experiment: A reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review78:3–43. [AK, rBFS, JERS]Google Scholar
Stent, G. (1981) Strength and weakness of the genetic appraoch to the development of the nervous system. Annual Review of Neuroscience4:163–94. [DW]Google Scholar
Stephens, D. W. (1981) The logic of risk-sensitive foraging preferences. Animal Behaviour29:628–29. [AK]Google Scholar
Stiers, M. & Silberberg, A. (1974) Lever-contact responses in rats: Auto-maintenance with and without a negative response-reinforcer dependency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior22:497–506. [AMC]Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S. (1963) Discrimination learning with and without “errors.”Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior6:1–27. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1898) Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Reviews, Monographic Supplement8:1–109. [JPH]Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1900a) The associative processes in animals. Biological Lectures from the Marine Biological Laboratory of Woods Hole, 1899, 7:69–91. [GMB]Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1900b) Instinct. Biological Lectures from the Marine Biological Laboratory of Woods Hole, 1899, 7:57–67. [GMB]Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1965) Animal intelligence. Hafner. 1st ed.1911. [GG]Google Scholar
Thorpe, W. H. (1951) The learning abilities of birds. Part I. Ibis93:1–52. [taBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Timberlake, W. (1983) Rats' responses to a moving object related to food or water: A behavior-systems analysis. Animal Learning and Behavior11:309–20. [GMB]Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1951) The study of instinct. Oxford University Press. [JDD, JPH]Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1953) The herring-gull's world. Collins. [taBFS, JERS]Google Scholar
Trapold, M. A. & Overmier, J. B. (1972) The second learning process in instrumental conditioning. In: Classical conditioning, vol. 2, Current research and theory, ed. Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [JERS]Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1957) The strategy of genes. Allen and Unwin. [AJP]Google Scholar
Waddington, C. H. (1960) Evolutionary adaptation. In: The evolution of life, ed. Tax, S.. University of Chicago Press. [HCP]Google Scholar
Wahlsten, D. (1978) Behavioral genetics and animal learning. In: Psychopharmacology of aversively motivated behavior, ed. Anisman, H. & Bignami, G.. Plenum Press. [DW]Google Scholar
Wahlsten, D. (1979) Some logical fallacies in the classical ethological point of view. Behavioral and Brain Sciences2:48–49. [DW]Google Scholar
Wassermann, G. D. (1978) Neurobiological theory of psychological phenomena. Macmillan and University Park Press. [GDW]Google Scholar
Wassermann, G. D. (1979) Reply to Popper's attack on epiphenomenalism. Mind88:572–75. [GDW]Google Scholar
Wassermann, G. D. (1981a) Book review of Teleology by Andrew Woodfield. Philosophia10:125–32. [GDW]Google Scholar
Wassermann, G. D. (1981b) On the nature of the theory of evolution. Philosophy of Science48:609–28. [GDW]Google Scholar
Wassermann, G. D. (1982a) Materialism and mentality. Review of Metaphysics35:715–29. [GDW]Google Scholar
Wassermann, G. D. (1982b) TIMA part 1. TIMA as a paradigm for the evolution of molecular complementarities and macromolecules. Journal of Theoretical Biology96:77–86. [GDW]Google Scholar
Wassermann, G. D. (1982c) TIMA part 2. TIMA-based instructive evolution of macromolecules and organs and structures. Journal of Theoretical Biology99:609–28. [GDW]Google Scholar
Wassermann, G. D. (1983a) Human behaviour and biology. Dialectica37:169–84. [GDW]Google Scholar
Wassermann, G. D. (1983b) Quantum mechanics and consciousness. Nature and System5:3–16. [GDW]Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1924) Behaviorism. W. W. Norton. [GMB, taBFS]Google Scholar
Webster, G. & Goodwin, B. (1981) History and structure in biology. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine25:39–62. [DW]Google Scholar
West, M. J., King, A. P. & Eastzer, D. H. (1981) The cowbird: Reflections on development from an unlikely source. American Scientist69:56–66. [DW]Google Scholar
Whitman, C. O. (1899) Animal behavior. Biological Lectures from the Marine Biological Laboratory of Woods Hole, 1898, 6:285–338. [GMB]Google Scholar
Williams, D. R. & Williams, H. (1969) Automaintenance in the pigeon: Sustained pecking despite contingent non-reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior12:511–20. [AMC]Google Scholar
Williams, G. C. (1966) Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton University Press. [MTG, JAH]Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1975) Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Harvard University Press. [GMB]Google Scholar
Wilson, E. O. (1978) On human nature. Harvard University Press. [AJP]Google Scholar
Woodfield, A. (1976) Teleology. Cambridge University Press. [GBW]Google Scholar
Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1965) Self-regulating systems in populations of animals. Science147:1543–48. [JPH, taBFS]Google Scholar