Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T07:38:00.811Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A science of culture: Clarifications and extensions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 November 2006

Alex Mesoudi*
Affiliation:
Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution and School of Psychology, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, FifeKY16 9JP, Scotland, United Kingdomwww.missouri.edu/~mesoudia/ Department of Anthropology, University of Missouri–Columbia, Columbia, MO65211, Canada and W. Maurice Young Centre for Applied Ethics, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z2, Canada www.st-andrews.ac.uk/~seal
Andrew Whiten*
Affiliation:
Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution and School of Psychology, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, FifeKY16 9JP, Scotland, United Kingdomwww.missouri.edu/~mesoudia/
Kevin N. Laland*
Affiliation:
Centre for Social Learning and Cognitive Evolution and School of Biology, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, FifeKY16 9TS, Scotland, United Kingdomwww.st-and.ac.uk/~aw2/

Abstract:

We are encouraged that the majority of commentators endorse our evolutionary framework for studying culture, and several suggest extensions. Here we clarify our position, dwelling on misunderstandings and requests for exposition. We reiterate that using evolutionary biology as a model for unifying the social sciences within a single synthetic framework can stimulate a more progressive and rigorous science of culture.

Type
Authors' Response
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbott, R. J., James, J. K., Milne, R. I. & Gillies, A. C. M. (2003) Plant introductions, hybridization and gene flow. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 358:1123–32. [arAM]Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. & Postman, L. (1947) The psychology of rumor. Henry Holt. [aAM]Google Scholar
Anderson, L. & Holt, C. (1997) Information cascades in the laboratory. American Economic Review 87:847–62. [BS]Google Scholar
Angier, N. (1997) Ernst Mayr at 93. Natural History 106(4):811. [MJO]Google Scholar
Arnold, S. J. & Wade, M. J. (1984) On the measurement of natural and sexual selection – theory. Evolution 38:709–19. [aAM]Google Scholar
Arthur, B. (1994) Increasing returns and path-dependence in the economy. Michigan University Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Arthur, W. (1997) The origin of animal body plans: A study in evolutionary developmental biology. Cambridge University Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Astuti, R., Soloman, G. A. & Carey, S., eds. (2004) Constraints on conceptual development: A case study of the acquisition of folkbiological and folksociological knowledge in Madagascar. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Blackwell. [JT]Google Scholar
Atran, S. (1998) Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(4):547–69. [aAM]Google Scholar
Atran, S. (2002) In god we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford University Press. [DK]Google Scholar
Aunger, R. A. (1995) On ethnography: Storytelling or science. Current Anthropology 36:97130. [rAM]Google Scholar
Aunger, R. A. (2000a) The life history of culture learning in a face-to-face society. Ethos 28:138. [arAM]Google Scholar
Aunger, R. A. ed. (2000b) Darwinizing culture. Oxford University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Aunger, R. A. (2002) The electric meme: A new theory of how we think. Simon and Schuster/Free Press. [RA, aAM, RLL, SB]Google Scholar
Avikainen, S., Kulomaki, T. & Hari, R. (1999) Normal movement reading in Asperger subjects. NeuroReport 10:3467–70. [aAM]Google Scholar
Avital, E. & Jablonka, E. (2000) Animal traditions: Behavioural inheritance in evolution. Cambridge University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Axelrod, R. (1997) The dissemination of culture: A model with local convergence and global polarization. Journal of Conflict Resolution 41:203–26. [rAM]Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1990) Human memory. Allyn and Bacon. [aAM]Google Scholar
Bandelt, H. J., Forster, P. & Rohl, A. (1999) Median-joining networks for inferring intraspecific phylogenies. Molecular Biological Evolution 16:3748. [rAM]Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977) Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall. [aAM]Google Scholar
Bandura, A., Ross, D. & Ross, S. A. (1961) Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 63:575–82. [rAM]Google Scholar
Banerjee, A. V. (2002) A simple model of herd behavior. Quarterly Journal of Economics 107:797817. [BS]Google Scholar
Bangerter, A. (2000) Transformation between scientific and social representations of conception: The method of serial reproduction. British Journal of Social Psychology 39:521–35. [rAM]Google Scholar
Bangerter, A. & Heath, C. (2004) The Mozart effect: Tracking the evolution of a scientific legend. British Journal of Social Psychology 43:605–23. [aAM]Google Scholar
Barbrook, A. C., Howe, C. J., Blake, N. & Robinson, P. (1998) The phylogeny of The Canterbury Tales. Nature 394:839. [aAM]Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (1989a) Broad training for social scientists. Science 243:992. [JHB]Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (1989b) Darwin, sex, and status: Biological approaches to mind and culture. University of Toronto Press. [JHB]Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H. (2006) Sometimes the bus does wait. In: Missing the revolution: Darwinism for social scientists, ed. Barkow, J. H., pp. 359. Oxford University Press. [JHB]Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., eds. (1992) The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Barrett, C. (2004) Design versus descent in Shuar children's reasoning about animals. Journal of Cognition and Culture 4:2550. [JT]Google Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932) Remembering. Macmillan. [aAM]Google Scholar
Basalla, G. (1988) The evolution of technology. Cambridge University Press. [arAM, WCW]Google Scholar
Bates, R., Grief, A., Levi, M., Rosenthal, J.-L. & Weingast, B. (1998) Analytic narratives. Princeton University Press. [HK]Google Scholar
Batterman, R. (2002) The devil in the details. Oxford University Press. [HK]Google Scholar
Baum, W. M., Richerson, P. J., Efferson, C. M. & Paciotti, B. M. (2004) Cultural evolution in laboratory micro-societies including traditions of rule-giving and rule-following. Evolution and Human Behavior 25:305–26. [arAM, SMR]Google Scholar
Begon, M., Harper, J. L. & Townsend, C. R. (1996) Ecology: Individuals, populations and communities. Blackwell. [aAM]Google Scholar
Beltman, J. B., Haccou, P. & ten Cate, C. (2003) The impact of learning foster species’ song on the evolution of specialist avian brood parasitism. Behavioral Ecology 14:917–23. [aAM]Google Scholar
Beltman, J. B., Haccou, P. & ten Cate, C. (2004) Learning and colonization of new niches: A first step toward speciation. Evolution 58:3546. [aAM]Google Scholar
Bennett, J. W. (1999) Classic anthropology. American Anthropologist 100:951–56. [aAM]Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A. & Shennan, S. J. (2003) Cultural transmission and stochastic network growth. American Antiquity 68:459–85. [arAM]Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A., Hahn, M. W. & Shennan, S. J. (2004) Random drift and culture change. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 271:1443–50. [aAM]Google Scholar
Bikhchandani, S., Hirschleifer, D. & Welch, I. (1992) A theory of fads, fashion, custom and cultural change as information cascades. Journal of Political Economy 100:9921026. [BS]Google Scholar
Blackmore, S. (1999) The meme machine. Oxford University Press. [BB, SB, arAM]Google Scholar
Blackmore, S. (2001) Evolution and memes: The human brain as a selective imitation device. Cybernetics and Systems 32:225–55. [SB]Google Scholar
Bliege Bird, R., Smith, E. A. & Bird, D. W. (2001) The hunting handicap: Costly signaling in male foraging strategies. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 50:919. [LC]Google Scholar
Bliss, T. V. P. & Lomo, T. (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. Journal of Physiology 232:331–56. [aAM]Google Scholar
Bloch, M. (2000) A well-disposed social anthropologist's problems with memes. In: Darwinizing culture, ed. Aunger, R., pp. 189204. Oxford University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Bloch, M. & Parry, J., eds. (1989) Money and the morality of exchange. Cambridge University Press. [JT]Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2001) Précis of How children learn the meanings of words. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(6):1095–103. [rAM]Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1896) The growth of Indian mythologies. Journal of American Folk-Lore 9:111. [RLL]Google Scholar
Bogdan, R. (2000) Minding minds: Evolving a reflexive mind in interpreting others. MIT Press. [TK]Google Scholar
Bordia, P. & Rosnow, R. L. (1998) Rumor rest stops on the information highway: Transmission patterns in a computer-mediated rumor chain. Human Communication Research 25:163–79. [aAM]Google Scholar
Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Nunn, C. L. & Towner, M. C. (2006) Macroevolutionary studies of cultural trait transmission. Evolutionary Anthropology 15(2):5264. [MBM]Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press. [BB, DK, arAM]Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (2000) Memes: Universal acid or a better mousetrap? In: Darwinising culture, ed. Aunger, R.. Oxford University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. (2005) The origin and evolution of cultures. Oxford University Press. [DK, arAM]Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (1994) Naturalness of religious ideas. University of California Press. [LC]Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001) Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books. [DK]Google Scholar
Bradburd, D. (1998) Being there: The necessity of fieldwork. Smithsonian Institution Press. [JT]Google Scholar
Brakefield, P. M. (2003) The power of evo-devo to explore evolutionary constraints: Experiments with butterfly eyespots. Zoology 106:283–90. [SMR]Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B. (2003) Psychology and evolution: The origins of mind. Sage. [BB]Google Scholar
Brown, J. H. & Lomolino, M. V. (1998) Biogeography. Sinauer. [aAM]Google Scholar
Brown, W. M., Cronk, L., Jacobson, A., Grochow, K., Liu, C. K., Popovic, Z. & Trivers, R. (2005) Dance reveals symmetry especially in young men. Nature 438:1148–50. [LC]Google Scholar
Bryant, J. M. (2004) An evolutionary social science? A skeptic's brief, theoretical and substantive. Philosophy of the Social Sciences 34:451–92. [aAM]Google Scholar
Burns, K. J., Hackett, S. J. & Klein, N. K. (2002) Phylogenetic relationships and morphological diversity in Darwin's finches and their relatives. Evolution 56:1240–52. [aAM]Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (1999) Imitation without intentionality. Using string parsing to copy the organisation of behaviour. Animal Cognition 2:6372. [aAM]Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W., Barnard, P. J., Davidson, I., Janik, V. M., McGrew, W. C., Miklosi, A. & Wiessner, P. (2004) Understanding culture across species. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8:341–46. [aAM]Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. & Russon, A. E. (1998) Learning by imitation: A hierarchical approach. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21(5):667–84. [aAM]Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960) Blind variation and selective retentions in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review 67:380400. [aAM]Google Scholar
Carlson, W. B. (2000) Invention and evolution: The case of Edison's sketches of the telephone. In: Technological innovation as an evolutionary process, ed. Ziman, J., pp. 137–58. Cambridge University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Carneiro, R. (1985) Comment on “Darwinian selection, symbolic variation, and the evolution of culture” by Rindos, D.. Current Anthropology 26(1):7778. [DWR]Google Scholar
Carroll, R. L. (2000) Towards a new evolutionary synthesis. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 15:2732. [rAM]Google Scholar
Carroll, S. B. (2001) The big picture. Nature 409:669. [DWR]Google Scholar
Carroll, S. B. (2005) Endless forms most beautiful: Evo-devo and the making of the animal kingdom. W. W. Norton. [WCW]Google Scholar
Catchpole, C. K. & Slater, P. J. B. (1995) Bird song: Biological themes and variations. Cambridge University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. (1981) Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton University Press. [arAM, MBM]Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Feldman, M. W., Chen, K. H. & Dornbusch, S. M. (1982) Theory and observation in cultural transmission. Science 218:1927. [aAM]Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., Minch, E. & Mountain, J. L. (1992) Coevolution of genes and languages revisited. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 89(12):5620–24. [MBM]Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Wang, W. S.-Y. (1986) Spatial distance and lexical replacement. Language 62:3855. [aAM]Google Scholar
Chagnon, N. A. (1988) Male Yanomamö manipulations of kinship classifications of female kin for reproductive advantage. In: Human reproductive behaviour: A Darwinian perspective, ed. Betzig, L., Borgerhoff Mulder, M. & Turke, P.. Cambridge University Press. [LC]Google Scholar
Chagnon, N. A. (2000) Manipulating kinship rules: A form of male Yanomamö reproductive competition. In: Adaptation and human behavior: An anthropological perspective, ed. Cronk, L., Irons, W., & Chagnon, N. A.. Aldine de Gruyter. [LC]Google Scholar
Chaudhuri, A., Schotter, A. & Sopher, B. (2005) Talking ourselves to efficiency: Coordination in intergenerational minimum games with private, almost common, and common knowledge of advice. Working paper, Rutgers University–New Brunswick, NJ. [BS]Google Scholar
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E. & Norenzayan, A. (1999) Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin 125:4763. [rAM]Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997) Being there: Putting brain, body and world together again. MIT Press. [HK]Google Scholar
Clifford, J. & Marcus, G., eds. (1986) Writing cultures: The poetics and politics of ethnography. University of California Press. [JT]Google Scholar
Cockburn, A. (1991) An introduction to evolutionary ecology. Blackwell. [aAM]Google Scholar
Coleman, J. S., Katz, E. & Menzel, H. (1966) Medical innovation: A diffusion study. Bobbs-Merrill. [aAM]Google Scholar
Collard, M., Shennan, S. & Tehrani, J. (2005) Branching versus blending in macroscale cultural evolution: A comparative study. In: Mapping our ancestors: Phylogenetic methods in anthropology and prehistory, ed. Lipo, C. P., O’Brien, M. J., Collard, M. & Shennan, S.. Aldine de Gruyter. [arAM, JT]Google Scholar
Collard, M., Shennan, S. & Tehrani, J. (2006) Branching, blending and the evolution of cultural similarities and differences in human populations. Evolution and Human Behavior 27(3):169–84. [JT]Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., Tooby, J. & Barkow, J. H. (1992) Introduction. In: The adapted mind. Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J.. Oxford University Press. [JHB]Google Scholar
Cronin, H. (1991) The ant and the peacock. Cambridge University Press. [DD]Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (1989) From hunters to herders: Subsistence change as a reproductive strategy among the Mukogodo. Current Anthropology 30:224–34. [LC]Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (1991) Human behavioral ecology. Annual Review of Anthropology 20:2553. [LC]Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (1994) Evolutionary theories of morality and the manipulative use of signals. Zygon: Journal of Religion and Science 29:81101. [LC]Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (1995) Is there a role for culture in human behavioral ecology? Evolution and Human Behavior 16:181205. [LC]Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (1999) That complex whole: Culture and the evolution of human behavior. Westview Press. [LC]Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (2002) From true Dorobo to Mukogodo Maasai: Contested ethnicity in Kenya. Ethnology 41:2749. [LC]Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (2004) From Mukogodo to Maasai: Ethnicity and cultural change in Kenya. Westview Press. [LC]Google Scholar
Cronk, L. (2005) The application of animal signaling theory to human phenomena: Some thoughts and clarifications. Social Science Information/Information sur les Sciences Sociales 44:603–20. [LC]Google Scholar
Cronk, L., Campbell, L. Milroy, A. & Simpson, J. A. (2002) Cosmetics as a signaling system. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Anthropological Association, New Orleans, LA. November 21–24, 2002. [LC]Google Scholar
Cronk, L. & Dunham, B. (2003) Engagement rings as signals in American courtship. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society, University of Nebraska, Lincoln, NE. June 4–8, 2003. [LC]Google Scholar
Crook, J. & Crook, S. J. (1988) Tibetan polyandry: Problems of adaptation and fitness. In: Human reproductive behaviour: A Darwinian perspective, ed. Betzig, L., Borgerhoff Mulder, M. & Turke, P., pp. 97114. Cambridge University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Crosson, B., Moberg, P. J., Boone, J. R., Rothi, L. J. G. & Raymer, A. (1997) Category-specific naming deficit for medical terms after dominant thalamic/ capsular hemorrhage. Brain and Language 60:407–42. [aAM]Google Scholar
Crow, J. F. (2001) The beanbag lives on. Nature 409:771–71. [aAM]Google Scholar
Crow, J. F. & Kimura, M. (1970) An introduction to population genetics theory. Harper & Row. [aAM]Google Scholar
Csibra, G. & Gergely, G. (2005) Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In: Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and performance, vol. XXI, ed. Johnson, M. H. & Munakata, Y.. Oxford University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Curio, E., Ernst, U. & Vieth, W. (1978) Cultural transmission of enemy recognition: One function of mobbing. Science 202:899901. [aAM]Google Scholar
Czerniak, C. (1981) The riddle of the universe and its solution. In: The mind's I, ed. Hofstadter, D. R. & Dennett, D. C., pp. 269–76. Bantam Books. [DB]Google Scholar
Dalton, R. (2000) Biologists flock to “evo-devo” in a quest to read the recipes of life. Nature 403:125. [rAM]Google Scholar
Daly, M. (1982) Some caveats about cultural transmission models. Human Ecology 10:401–08. [rAM]Google Scholar
Danchin, E., Giraldeau, L. A., Valone, T. J. & Wagner, R. H. (2004) Public information: From nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science 305:487–91. [aAM]Google Scholar
D’Andrade, R. (2001) A cognitivist's view of the units debated in cultural anthropology. Cross-Cultural Research 35(2):242–57. [DWR]Google Scholar
Danielson, P. (2004) Rationality and evolution. In: The Oxford handbook of rationality, ed. Mele, A. R. & Rawling, P., pp. 417–37. Oxford University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859/1968) The origin of species. Penguin. [arAM]Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871/2003) The descent of man. Gibson Square. [aAM]Google Scholar
Davidson, E. (2001) Genomic regulatory systems: Evolution and development. Academic. [WCW]Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976) The selfish gene. Oxford University Press. [SB, aAM]Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1982) The extended phenotype. Oxford University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1983) Universal Darwinism. In: Evolution from molecules to man, ed. Bendall, D. S., pp. 403–25. Cambridge University Press. [TK]Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1995) River out of Eden. Basic Books. [rAM]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown. [SB]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1995) Darwin's dangerous idea. Penguin/Simon & Schuster. [SB, DD, aAM]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2001) The evolution of culture. The Monist 84:305–24. [aAM]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2002) The new replicators. In: The encyclopedia of evolution, vol. 1, ed. Pagel, M., pp. E83–E92. Oxford University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Diamond, J. (1978) The Tasmanians: The longest isolation, the simplest technology. Nature 273:185–86. [aAM]Google Scholar
Diamond, J. (1998) Guns, germs and steel. Vintage. [aAM]Google Scholar
Diamond, J. (1999) Guns, germs, and steel: The fates of human societies. W. W. Norton. [WM]Google Scholar
Diamond, J. (2005) Collapse: How societies choose to fail or succeed. Viking Press. [WM]Google Scholar
Dobzhansky, T., Ayala, F. J., Stebbins, G. L. & Valentine, J. W. (1977) Evolution. Freeman. [aAM]Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991) Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press. [RA, BB, arAM]Google Scholar
Donley, J. M., Sepulveda, C. A., Konstantinidis, P., Gemballa, S. & Shadwick, R. E. (2004) Convergent evolution in mechanical design of lamnid sharks and tunas. Nature 429:6165. [aAM]Google Scholar
Doolittle, W. F. (1999) Phylogenetic classification and the universal tree. Science 284:2124–28. [arAM]Google Scholar
Dow, M., Burton, M., White, D. R. & Reitz, K. (1984) Galton's problem as a network autocorrelation. American Ethnologist 11:754–70. [aAM]Google Scholar
DuBois, C. A. (1939) The 1870 Ghost Dance. University of California Anthropological Records 3(1):1151. [RLL]Google Scholar
Dunwiddie, T. & Lynch, G. (1978) Long-term potentiation and depression of synaptic responses in the rat hippocampus: Localisation and frequency dependency. Journal of Physiology 276:353–67. [aAM]Google Scholar
Durham, W. H. (1990) Advances in evolutionary culture theory. Annual Review of Anthropology 19:187210. [JT]Google Scholar
Durham, W. H. (1992a) Applications of evolutionary culture theory. Annual Review of Anthropology 21:331–55. [JT]Google Scholar
Durham, W. H. (1992b) Coevolution: Genes, culture, and human diversity. Stanford University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Earl, D. J. & Deem, M. W. (2004) Evolvability is a selectable trait. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 32:11531–36. [SMR]Google Scholar
Eerkens, J. W., Bettinger, R. L. & McElreath, R. (2006) Cultural transmission, phylogenetics, and the archaeological record. In: Mapping our ancestors: Phylogenetic methods in anthropology and prehistory, ed. Lipo, C. P., O’Brien, M. J., Collard, M. & Shennan, S. J., pp. 169–83. Aldine de Gruyter. [rAM, MBM]Google Scholar
Eldredge, N. & Gould, S. J. (1972) Punctuated equilibria: An alternative to phyletic gradualism. In: Models in paleobiology, ed. Schopf, T. J. M.. Freeman, Cooper. [MJO]Google Scholar
Enard, W. & Pääbo, S. (2004) Comparative primate genetics. Annual Review of Genomics and Human Genetics 5:351–78. [MBM]Google Scholar
Endler, J. A. (1986) Natural selection in the wild. Princeton University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Epstein, J. M. & Axtell, R. (1996) Growing artificial societies: Social science from the bottom up. MIT Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Erwin, D. (2000) Macroevolution is more than repeated rounds of microevolution. Evolution and Development 2(2):7884. [DWR]Google Scholar
Evans, J. (1850) On the date of British coins. The Numismatic Chronicle and Journal of the Numismatic Society 12:127–37. [aAM]Google Scholar
Ewald, P. (1994) Evolution of infectious disease. Oxford University Press. [JHB]Google Scholar
Feldman, M. W. & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1976) Cultural and biological evolutionary processes, selection for a trait under complex transmission. Theoretical Population Biology 9:238–59. [aAM]Google Scholar
Feldman, M. W. & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1989) On the theory of evolution under genetic and cultural transmission with application to the lactose absorption problem. In: Mathematical evolutionary theory, ed. Feldman, M. W.. Princeton University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Fessler, D. M. T. & Navarrete, C. D. (2003) Meat is good to taboo: Dietary proscriptions as a product of the interaction of psychological mechanisms and social processes. Journal of Cognition and Culture 3(1):140. [DK]Google Scholar
Fisher, R. A. (1930) The genetical theory of natural selection. Clarendon Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983) The modularity of mind. MIT Press. [SMR]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2000) The mind doesn't work that way: The scope and limits of computational psychology. MIT Press. [SMR]Google Scholar
Forster, P. & Toth, A. (2003) Toward a phylogenetic chronology of ancient Gaulish, Celtic and Indo-European. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100:9079–84. [rAM]Google Scholar
Fragaszy, D. M. & Perry, S., eds. (2003) The biology of traditions: Models and evidence. Cambridge University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Frank, S. A. (1998) Foundations of social evolution. Princeton University Press. [TK]Google Scholar
Futuyma, D. J. (1998) Evolutionary biology. Sinauer. [arAM]Google Scholar
Gaddis, J. (2002) The landscape of history. Oxford University Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Gale, D. & Kariv, S. (2003) Bayesian learning in social networks. Games and Economic Behavior 45:329–46. [BS]Google Scholar
Galef, B. G. & Allen, C. (1995) A new model for studying behavioural traditions in animals. Animal Behaviour 50:705–17. [aAM]Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Rizzolatti, G. (1996) Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain 119:593609. [aAM]Google Scholar
Gallese, V. & Goldman, A. (1998) Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Science 2:493501. [aAM]Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1889) Discussion following E. B. Tylor's “On a method of investigating the development of institutions: Applied to laws of marriage and descent.” Journal of the Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 18:245272. [aAM]Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973) The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays. Basic Books. [JT]Google Scholar
Gerard, R. W., Kluckhohn, C. & Rapoport, A. (1956) Biological and cultural evolution: Some analogies and explorations. Behavioral Science 1:634. [aAM]Google Scholar
Gergely, G. & Csibra, G. (2006) Sylvia's recipe: The role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge. In: Roots of human sociality: Culture, cognition, and human interaction, ed. Enfield, N. J. & Levenson, S. C.. Berg. [rAM]Google Scholar
Gerkey, A. & Cronk, L. (2005) Human behavioral ecology and ethnography: Dialogues between behavior and culture. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Human Behavior and Evolution Society, Austin, TX. June 1–5, 2005. [LC]Google Scholar
Gilbert, S. F. (2000) Developmental biology. Sinauer. [rAM]Google Scholar
Gil-White, F. (2001) Are ethnic groups biological “species” to the human brain? Essentialism in our cognition of some social categories. Current Anthropology 43(4):515–54. [DK]Google Scholar
Gintis, H. (2000) Game theory evolving. Princeton University Press. [HK, rAM]Google Scholar
Goldenberg, G. & Hermsdorfer, J. (2002) Imitation, apraxia, and hemispheric dominance. In: The imitative mind, ed. Meltzoff, A. N. & Prinz, W., pp. 331346. Cambridge University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Goodwin, N. B., Balshine-Earn, S. & Reynolds, J. D. (1998) Evolutionary transitions in parental care in cichlid fish. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 265:2265–72. [aAM]Google Scholar
Goody, J. (1982) Cooking, cuisine and class. Cambridge University Press. [JHB]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1977) Ontogeny and phylogeny. Harvard University Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1980) The panda's thumb. W. W. Norton. [aAM]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1987) The panda's thumb of technology. Natural History 96(1):1423. [MJO]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1991) Bully for brontosaurus. W. W. Norton. [aAM]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1996) Full house: The spread of excellence from Plato to Darwin. Harmony Press. [rAM, MJO]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1997) A tale of two worksites. Natural History 106(9):1822, 29, 62, 64–68. [rAM, MJO]Google Scholar
Goyal, S. (2005) Learning in networks. In: Group formation in economics: Networks, clubs and coalitions, ed. Demange, G. & Wooders, M.. Cambridge University Press. [BS]Google Scholar
Gray, R. D. & Atkinson, Q. D. (2003) Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin. Nature 426:435–39. [aAM, MP]Google Scholar
Gray, R. D. & Jordan, F. M. (2000) Language trees support the express-train sequence of Austronesian expansion. Nature 405:1052–55. [aAM]Google Scholar
Griesemer, J. (2005) Genetics from an evolutionary process perspective. In: Genes in development, ed. Neumann-Held, E. M. & Rehmann-Sutter, C.. Duke University Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Grimes, B. F. (2002) Ethnologue: Languages of the world, 14th edition. Summer Institute of Linguistics. [aAM]Google Scholar
Guglielmino, C. R., Viganotti, C., Hewlett, B. & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1995) Cultural variation in Africa: Role of mechanisms of transmission and adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 92:585–89. [arAM]Google Scholar
Guilford, T. & Dawkins, M. S. (1991) Receiver psychology and the evolution of animal signals. Animal Behaviour 42:114. [LC]Google Scholar
Haldane, J. B. S. (1932) The causes of evolution. Longmans, Green. [aAM]Google Scholar
Haldane, J. B. S. (1964) A defense of beanbag genetics. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 7:343–59. [aAM]Google Scholar
Hallpike, C. R. (1986) The principles of social evolution. Clarendon Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Hannan, M. T. & Freeman, J. (1989) Organizational ecology. Harvard University Press. [HK, rAM, TK]Google Scholar
Hansen, T. F. (2003) Is modularity necessary for evolvability? Remarks on the relationship between pleiotropy and evolvability. BioSystems 69:8394. [SMR]Google Scholar
Hartl, D. L. & Clark, A. G. (1997) Principles of population genetics. Sinauer. [aAM]Google Scholar
Harvey, P. H. & Pagel, M. D. (1991) The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Hatch, E. (1973) Theories of man and culture. Columbia University Press. [RLL]Google Scholar
Hauser, M. (1996) The evolution of animal communication. MIT Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Heath, C., Bell, C. & Sternberg, E. (2001) Emotional selection in memes: The case of urban legends. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81:1028–41. [DK]Google Scholar
Hebb, D. O. (1949) The organization of behaviour. Wiley. [aAM]Google Scholar
Helfman, G. S. & Schultz, E. T. (1984) Social transmission of behavioral traditions in a coral-reef fish. Animal Behaviour 32:379–84. [arAM]Google Scholar
Hendry, A. P. & Kinnison, M. T. (1999) The pace of modern life: Measuring rates of contemporary microevolution. Evolution 53:1637–53. [aAM]Google Scholar
Hennig, W. (1966) Phylogenetic systematics. University of Illinois Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2004a) Cultural group selection, coevolutionary processes and large-scale cooperation. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 53(1):335. [TK]Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2004b) Demography and cultural evolution: How adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses – the Tasmanian case. American Antiquity 69:197214. [aAM]Google Scholar
Henrich, J. & Boyd, R. (1998) The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior 19:215–41. [aAM]Google Scholar
Henrich, J. & Boyd, R. (2002) On modeling cognition and culture: Why cultural evolution does not require replication of representations. Journal of Cognition and Culture 2:87112. [aAM]Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E. & Gintis, H., eds. (2004) Foundations of human sociality: Economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from 15 small-scale societies. Oxford University Press. [JT]Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., Gintis, H., McElreath, R., Alvard, M., Barr, A., Ensminger, J., Smith Henrich, N., Hill, K., Gil-White, F., Gurven, M., Marlowe, F. W., Patton, J. Q. & Tracer, D. (2005) Economic man in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioural experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(6):795815. [rAM, JT]Google Scholar
Henrich, J. & Gil-White, F. (2001) The evolution of prestige: Freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 22:165–96. [DK]Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S. & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1986) Cultural transmission among Aka pygmies. American Anthropologist 88:922–34. [arAM, MBM, JT]Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., De Silvestri, A. & Guglielmino, C. R. (2002) Semes and genes in Africa. Current Anthropology 43:313–21. [arAM]Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M. (2005) Imitation by association. In: Perspectives on imitation, ed. Hurley, S. & Chater, N.. MIT Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Hinde, R. A. & Barden, L. A. (1985) The evolution of the teddy bear. Animal Behaviour 33:1371–73. [aAM]Google Scholar
Hodgson, G. M. (2004) The evolution of institutional economics: Agency, structure and Darwinism in American institutionalism. Routledge. [TK]Google Scholar
Hodgson, G. M. & Knudsen, T. (in press a) Dismantling Lamarckism: Why descriptions of socio-economic evolution as Lamarckian are misleading. Journal of Evolutionary Economics. DOI: 10.1007/s00191-006-0019-3 (Published online 30 March, 2006). [TK]Google Scholar
Hodgson, G. M. & Knudsen, T. (in press b) Why we need a generalized Darwinism: And why a generalized Darwinism is not enough. JJournal of Economic Behavior and OrganizationJ. DOI: 10.1016/j.jebo.2005.01.004 (Published online 4 November, 2005). [TK]Google Scholar
Hoekstra, H. E., Hoekstra, J. M., Berrigan, D., Vignieri, S. N., Hoang, A., Hill, C. E., Beerli, P. & Kingsolver, J. G. (2001) Strength and tempo of directional selection in the wild. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98:9157–60. [aAM]Google Scholar
Holden, C. J. (2002) Bantu language trees reflect the spread of farming across sub- Saharan Africa: A maximum-parsimony analysis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 269:793–99. [aAM]Google Scholar
Holden, C. J. & Mace, R. (2003) Spread of cattle led to the loss of matrilineal descent in Africa: A coevolutionary analysis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 270:2425–33. [aAM]Google Scholar
Horner, V. & Whiten, A. (2005) Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition 8:164–81. [rAM]Google Scholar
Howe, C. J., Barbrook, A. C., Spencer, M., Robinson, P., Bordalejo, B. & Mooney, L. R. (2001) Manuscript evolution. Trends in Genetics 17:147–52. [aAM]Google Scholar
Hull, D. L. (1982) The naked meme. In: Learning, development and culture: Essays in evolutionary epistemology, ed. Plotkin, H. C., pp. 273327. John Wiley. [aAM, TK]Google Scholar
Hull, D. L. (2001) Science and selection: Essays on biological evolution and the philosophy of science. Cambridge University Press. [TK]Google Scholar
Hull, D. L., Langman, R. E. & Glenn, S. S. (2001) A general account of selection: Biology, immunology, and behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24(3):511–28. [aAM, TK]Google Scholar
Hurley, S. & Chater, N., eds. (2005) Perspectives on imitation. MIT Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995) Cognition in the wild. MIT Press. [RA, HK]Google Scholar
Huxley, J. S. (1942) Evolution, the modern synthesis. Allen & Unwin. [rAM, MJO]Google Scholar
Huxley, J. S. (1955) Evolution, cultural and biological. Yearbook of Anthropology 225. [aAM]Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M. (2005) Neural mechanisms of imitation. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 15:632–37. [rAM]Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M., Koski, L. M., Brass, M., Bekkering, H.,Woods, R. P., Dubeau, M.-C., Mazziotta, J. C. & Rizzolatti, G. (2001) Reafferent copies of imitated actions in the right superior temporal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98:13995–99. [aAM]Google Scholar
Insko, C. A., Thibaut, J. W., Moehle, D., Wilson, M., Diamond, W. D. & Gilmore, R. (1980) Social evolution and the emergence of leadership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39:431–48. [rAM]Google Scholar
Irons, W. (1996) In our own self image: The evolution of morality, deception, and religion. Skeptic 4:5061. [LC]Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. & Lamb, M. (2005) Evolution in four dimensions. MIT Press [WCW]Google Scholar
Jackson, M. & Watts, A. (2002) The evolution of social and economic networks. Journal of Economic Theory 106:265–95. [BS]Google Scholar
Jacobs, R. C. & Campbell, D. T. (1961) The perpetuation of an arbitrary tradition through several generations of a laboratory microculture. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 62:649–58. [aAM]Google Scholar
Jaeger, M. E., Anthony, S. & Rosnow, R. L. (1980) Who hears what from whom and with what effect: A study of rumor. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 6:473–78. [aAM]Google Scholar
James, W. (1880) Great men, great thoughts, and the environment. Atlantic Monthly 46:441–59. [aAM]Google Scholar
Janik, V. M. & Slater, P. J. B. (1997) Vocal learning in mammals. Advances in the Study of Behavior 26:5999. [aAM]Google Scholar
Johnson, G. R. (1986) Kin selection, socialization, and patriotism: An integrating theory. Politics and the Life Sciences 4:127–54. [LC]Google Scholar
Johnson, G. R. (1987) In the name of the fatherland: An analysis of kin term usage in patriotic speech and literature. International Political Science Review 8:165–74. [LC]Google Scholar
Johnson, G. R. (1989) The role of kin recognition mechanisms in patriotic socialization: Further reflections. Politics and the Life Sciences 8:6269. [LC]Google Scholar
Jordan, P. & Shennan, S. (2003) Cultural transmission, language, and basketry traditions amongst the Californian Indians. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 22:4274. [JT]Google Scholar
Kashima, Y. (2000) Maintaining cultural stereotypes in the serial reproduction of narratives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 26:594604. [rAM]Google Scholar
Kawai, M. (1965) Newly acquired pre-cultural behavior of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islet. Primates 6:130. [aAM]Google Scholar
Kennington, W. J., Killeen, J. R., Goldstein, D. B. & Partridge, L. (2003) Rapid laboratory evolution of adult wing area in Drosophila melanogaster in response to humidity. Evolution 57:932–36. [aAM]Google Scholar
Keysers, C. & Perrett, D. I. (2004) Demystifying social cognition: A Hebbian perspective. Trends in Cognitive Science 8:501507. [aAM]Google Scholar
Kidder, A. V. (1932) The artifacts of Pecos. Phillips Academy, Papers of the Southwestern Expedition, No. 6. [MJO]Google Scholar
Kincaid, H. (1996) Philosophical foundations of the social sciences. Cambridge University Press. [HK]Google Scholar
Kincaid, H. (2006) Functional explanation and evolutionary social science. In: Handbook of philosophy of science, vol. 15: Philosophy of anthropology and sociology, ed. Turner, S. & Risjord, M.. Elsevier. [HK]Google Scholar
Kingsolver, J. G., Hoekstra, H. E., Hoekstra, J. M., Berrigan, D., Vignieri, S. N., Hill, C. E., Hoang, A., Gibert, P. & Beerli, P. (2001) The strength of phenotypic selection in natural populations. American Naturalist 157:245–61. [aAM]Google Scholar
Kirschner, M. & Gerhart, J. (1998) Evolvability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 95:8420–27. [SMR]Google Scholar
Knudsen, T. (2004) General selection theory and economic evolution: The price equation and the replicator/interactor distinction. Journal of Economic Methodology 11(2):147–73. [TK]Google Scholar
Kohler, T. A. & Gumerman, G. J., eds. (2000) Dynamics in human and primate societies: Agent-based modeling of social and spatial processes. Oxford University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Korpimäki, E., Norrdahl, K., Klemola, T., Pettersen, T. & Stenseth, N. C. (2002) Dynamic effects of predators on cyclic voles: Field experimentation and model extrapolation. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 269:991–97. [aAM]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B., eds. (1997) Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach. Blackwell. [rAM]Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R. & Dawkins, R. (1984) Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In: Behavioural ecology: An evolutionary approach, ed. Krebs, J. R. & Davies, N. B.. Blackwell. [rAM]Google Scholar
Kuper, A. (1999) Culture: The anthropologist's account. Harvard University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Kutschera, U. & Niklas, K. J. (2004) The modern theory of biological evolution: An expanded synthesis. Naturwissenschaften 91:255–76. [rAM]Google Scholar
Lachlan, R. F. & Slater, P. J. B. (1999) The maintenance of vocal learning by gene-culture interaction: The cultural trap hypothesis. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B: Biological Sciences 266:701706. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. (1993) The mathematical modelling of human culture and its implications for psychology and the human sciences. British Journal of Psychology 84:145–69. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Bateson, P. (2001) The mechanisms of imitation. Cybernetics and Systems 32:195224. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Brown, G. R. (2002) Sense and nonsense: Evolutionary perspectives on human behaviour. Oxford University Press. [arAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Hoppitt, W. (2003) Do animals have culture? Evolutionary Anthropology 12:150–59. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Kendal, J. R. (2003) What the models say about social learning. In: The biology of traditions: Models and evidence, ed. Fragaszy, D. & Perry, S., pp. 3355. Chicago University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Kumm, J. & Feldman, M. W. (1995a) Gene-culture coevolutionary theory – a test-case. Current Anthropology 36:131–56. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Kumm, J., Vanhorn, J. D. & Feldman, M. W. (1995b) A gene-culture model of human handedness. Behavior Genetics 25:433–45. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J. & Feldman, M. W. (2000) Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23(1):131–46. [arAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Plotkin, H. C. (1990) Social learning and social transmission of foraging information in Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). Animal Learning and Behavior 18:246–51. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Plotkin, H. C. (1993) Social transmission of food preferences among Norway rats by marking of food sites and by gustatory contact. Animal Learning and Behavior 21:3541. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Williams, K. (1997) Shoaling generates social learning of foraging information in guppies. Animal Behaviour 53:1161–69. [aAM]Google Scholar
Laland, K. N. & Williams, K. (1998) Social transmission of maladaptive information in the guppy. Behavioral Ecology 9:493–99. [arAM]Google Scholar
Lande, R. & Arnold, S. J. (1983) The measurement of selection on correlated characters. Evolution 37:1210–26. [aAM]Google Scholar
Lefebvre, L. (1995) Culturally transmitted feeding behaviour in primates: Evidence for accelerating learning rates. Primates 36:227–39. [aAM]Google Scholar
Leonard, R. D. & Jones, G. T. (1987) Elements of an inclusive evolutionary model for archaeology. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 6:199–219. [MJO]Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (1970) The units of selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 1:118. [rAM, WCW]Google Scholar
Lumsden, C. J. & Wilson, E. O. (1981) Genes, mind and culture. Harvard University Press. [SB]Google Scholar
Lyman, R. L. (in press) Cultural transmission in North American anthropology and archaeology, ca. 1895–1965. In: Cultural transmission and archaeology: Issues and case studies, ed. O’Brien, M. J.. Society for American Archaeology. [RLL]Google Scholar
Lyman, R. L. & O’Brien, M. J. (1997) The concept of evolution in early twentiethcentury Americanist archaeology. In: Rediscovering Darwin: Evolutionary theory and archaeological explanation, ed. Barton, C. M. & Clark, G.. American Anthropological Association, Archeological Papers, No. 7. [MJO]Google Scholar
Lyman, R. L. & O’Brien, M. J. (1998) The goals of evolutionary archaeology: History and explanation. Current Anthropology 39:615–52. [RLL, MJO]Google Scholar
Lyman, R. L. & O’Brien, M. J. (2003) Cultural traits: Units of analysis in early twentieth-century anthropology. Journal of Anthropological Research 59:225–50. [RLL]Google Scholar
Lyman, R. L., O’Brien, M. J. & Dunnell, R. C. (1997) The rise and fall of culture history. Plenum. [MJO]Google Scholar
Lynch, A. & Baker, A. J. (1993) A population memetics approach to culturalevolution in chaffinch song: Meme diversity within populations. American Naturalist 141:597620. [aAM]Google Scholar
MacArthur, R. H. & Wilson, E. O. (1967) The theory of island biogeography. Princeton University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Mace, R. & Holden, C. J. (2005) A phylogenetic approach to cultural evolution. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 20:116–21. [aAM]Google Scholar
Mace, R. & Pagel, M. (1994) The comparative method in anthropology. Current Anthropology 35:549–64. [BB, arAM, MP]Google Scholar
Machery, E. & Faucher, L. (2005) Why do we think racially? In: Handbook of categorization in cognitive science, ed. Cohen, H. & Lefebvre, C.. Elsevier. [DK]Google Scholar
Machery, E. & Faucher, L. (2005) (forthcoming) Social construction and the concept of race. Philosophy of Science. [DK]Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. F. & Davis, B. L. (2000) On the origin of internal structure of word forms. Science 288:527–31. [aAM]Google Scholar
Maddison, W. P. & Maddison, D. R. (1992) MacClade version 3. Sinauer. [aAM]Google Scholar
Maienschein, J. & Laublicher, M. (2006) From embryology to evo-devo. MIT Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Mameli, M. (2001) Mindreading, mindshaping and evolution. Biology and Philosophy 16:597628. [rAM]Google Scholar
Marko, P. B. (2005) An intraspecific comparative analysis of character divergence between sympatric species. Evolution 59:554–64. [aAM]Google Scholar
Marshall, L. (1976) The !Kung of Nyae Nyae. Harvard University Press. [DWR]Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1963) Animal species and evolution. Harvard University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1982) The growth of biological thought: Diversity, evolution, and inheritance. Harvard University Press. [arAM, MJO]Google Scholar
Mayr, E. & Provine, W. B., eds. (1980) The evolutionary synthesis. Harvard University Press. [rAM, WCW]Google Scholar
McElreath, R. (1997) Iterated parsimony: A method for reconstructing cultural histories. M.Sc. thesis, Department of Anthropology, University of California at Los Angeles. [MBM]Google Scholar
McElreath, R., Lubell, M., Richerson, P., Waring, T., Baum, W., Edsten, E., Efferson, C. & Paciotti, B. (2005) Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior 26: 483508. [DK, rAM]Google Scholar
McGrew, W. C. (1987) Tools to get food: The subsistents of Tasmanian aborigines and Tanzanian chimpanzees compared. Journal of Anthropological Research 43:247–58. [aAM]Google Scholar
McKone, M. J. & Halpern, S. L. (2003) The evolution of androgenesis. American Naturalist 161:641–56. [aAM]Google Scholar
Medawar, P. B. (1982) Pluto's republic. Oxford University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Decety, J. (2003) What imitation tells us about social cognition: A rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B 358:491500. [aAM]Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1997) Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting 6:179–92. [aAM]Google Scholar
Mendel, G. (1866) Versuche über pflanzen-hybriden. Verhandlungen des Naturforschenden Vereines, Bru¨nn 4:347. [aAM]Google Scholar
Mercader, J., Panger, M. & Boesch, C. (2002) Excavation of a chimpanzee stone tool site in the African rainforest. Science 296:1452–55. [aAM]Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2005) The transmission and evolution of human culture. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, United Kingdom. [rAM]Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. & Whiten, A. (2004) The hierarchical transformation of event knowledge in human cultural transmission. Journal of Cognition and Culture 4:124. [arAM]Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A. & Dunbar, R. (2006) A bias for social information in human cultural transmission. British Journal of Psychology 97(3):405–23. [arAM, SMR]Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A. & Laland, K. N. (2004) Is human cultural evolution Darwinian? Evidence reviewed from the perspective of The Origin of Species. Evolution 58:111. [arAM, MP]Google Scholar
Midgley, M. (2000) Why memes? In: Alas, poor Darwin: Arguments against evolutionary psychology, ed. Rose, H. & Rose, S., pp. 6784. Jonathan Cape. [aAM]Google Scholar
Miller, G. (2000) The mating mind: How sexual choice shaped the evolution of human nature. Doubleday. [DD, DK]Google Scholar
Modrek, B. & Lee, C. (2002) A genomic view of alternative splicing. Nature Genetics 30:1319. [aAM]Google Scholar
Mooney, J. (1896) The Ghost Dance religion and the Sioux outbreak of 1890. Fourteenth Annual Report of the Bureau of American Ethnology for the Years 1892–1893, pp. 6411136. Bureau of American Ethnology. [RLL]Google Scholar
Moore, J. H. (1994) Putting anthropology back together again: The ethnogenetic critique of cladistic theory. American Anthropologist 96:925–48. [aAM]Google Scholar
Morgan, L. H. (1877) Ancient society. Holt. [rAM, MJO]Google Scholar
Morgan, M. & Morrison, M., eds. (1999) Models as mediators. Cambridge University Press. [HK]Google Scholar
Mousseau, T. A. & Fox, C. W. (1998) Maternal effects as adaptations. Oxford University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Mufwene, S. S. (2001) The ecology of language evolution. Cambridge University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1967) Ethnographic atlas. University of Pittsburgh Press. [aAM, JT]Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P., Ford, C., Hudson, A., Kennedy, R., Simmons, L. & Whiting, J. (1987) Outline of cultural materials (with modifications), 5th edition. Human Relations Area Files. [aAM]Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. & White, D. R. (1969) Standard cross-cultural sample. Ethnology 8:329–69. [aAM]Google Scholar
Nagell, K., Olguin, R. S. & Tomasello, M. (1993) Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology 107:174–86. [rAM]Google Scholar
Nehaniv, C. L. & Dautenhahn, K. (2002) The correspondence problem. In: Imitation in animals and artifacts, ed. Dautenhahn, K. & Nehaniv, C. L.. MIT Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Neiman, F. D. (1995) Stylistic variation in evolutionary perspective: Inferences from decorative diversity and interassemblage distance in Illinois woodland ceramic assemblages. American Antiquity 60:736. [arAM]Google Scholar
Nelson, R. R. & Winter, S. G. (1982) An evolutionary theory of economic change. Harvard University Press/Belknap Press. [TK, rAM, BS]Google Scholar
Nesse, R. & Williams, G. (1994) Why we get sick: The new science of Darwinian medicine. Times Books/Random House. [JHB]Google Scholar
Nichols, S. (2002) On the genealogy of norms: A case for the role of emotion in cultural evolution. Philosophy of Science 69:234–55. [DK]Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I. & Norenzayan, A. (2001) Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review 108:291310. [rAM]Google Scholar
Norman, D. A. (1988) The psychology of everyday things. Basic Books. [RA]Google Scholar
North, D. C. (1978) Structure and performance: The task of economic history. Journal of Economic Literature 16:963–78. [BS]Google Scholar
Nunn, C. L., Borgerhoff Mulder, M. & Langley, S. (2006) Comparative methods for studying cultural trait evolution: A simulation study. Cross-Cultural Research 40(2):177209. [rAM, MBM]Google Scholar
Nyarko, Y., Schotter, A. & Sopher, B. (forthcoming) On the informational content of advice: A theoretical and experimental study. Economic Theory. [BS]Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J. (1996) The historical development of an evolutionary archaeology. In: Darwinian archaeologies, ed. Maschner, H. D. G.. Plenum. [MJO]Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J., Darwent, J. & Lyman, R. L. (2001) Cladistics is useful for reconstructing archaeological phylogenies: Palaeoindian points from the southeastern United States. Journal of Archaeological Science 28:1115–36. [aAM]Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J. & Holland, T. D. (1990) Variation, selection, and the archaeological record. Archaeological Method and Theory 2:3179. [MJO]Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J. & Lyman, R. L. (2000) Applying evolutionary archaeology: A systematic approach. Kluwer Academic/Plenum. [arAM, MJO]Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J. & Lyman, R. L. (2002a) Darwinian evolutionism in archaeology: Current status and prospects for synthesis. Evolutionary Anthropology 11:2636. [RLL]Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J. & Lyman, R. L. (2002b) Evolutionary archaeology: Current status and future prospects. Evolutionary Anthropology 11:2636. [aAM, MJO]Google Scholar
O’Brien, M. J. & Lyman, R. L. (2003) Cladistics and archaeology. University of Utah Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Odling Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N. & Feldman, M. (2003) Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press. [RA, AF, rAM, WCW]Google Scholar
Ohmagari, K. & Berkes, F. (1997) Transmission of indigenous knowledge and bush skills among the Western James Bay Cree women of subarctic Canada. Human Ecology 25:197222. [rAM]Google Scholar
Orgel, L. & Crick, F. (1980) Selfish DNA: The ultimate parasite. Nature 284:604–07. [rAM]Google Scholar
Oyama, S., Griffiths, P. & Gray, R., eds. (2001) Cycles of contingency: Developmental systems and evolution. MIT Press. [AF, WCW]Google Scholar
Pagel, M. (1994) The adaptationist wager. In: Phylogenetics and ecology, ed. Eggleton, P. & Vane-Wright, R. I., pp. 2951. Academic Press. [MP]Google Scholar
Pagel, M. (2000) The history, rate, and pattern of world linguistic evolution. In: The evolutionary emergence of language, ed. Knight, C., Studdert-Kennedy, M. & Hurford, J., pp. 391416. Cambridge University Press. [MP]Google Scholar
Pagel, M. & Mace, R. (2004) The cultural wealth of nations. Nature 428:275–78. [aAM, MP]Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. & Panksepp, J. B. (2000) The seven sins of evolutionary psychology. Evolution and Cognition 6:108–31. [SMR]Google Scholar
Perrin, N. (1979) Giving up the gun. Hall. [aAM]Google Scholar
Perry, S., Baker, M., Fedigan, L., Gros-Louis, J., Jack, K., MacKinnon, K. C., Manson, J. H., Panger, M., Pyle, K. & Rose, L. (2003a) Social conventions in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys – evidence for traditions in a neotropical primate. Current Anthropology 44:241–68. [arAM]Google Scholar
Perry, S., Panger, M., Rose, L. M., Baker, M., Gros-Luis, J. & Jack, K. (2003b) Traditions in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys. In: The biology of traditions: Models and evidence, ed. Fragaszy, D. & Perry, S., pp. 391425. Cambridge University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Pétrequin, P. & Pétrequin, A. M. (1999) La poterie en Nouvelle-Guinee: Savoirefaire et transmission des techniques. Journal de la Société des Océanistes 108:71101. [JT]Google Scholar
Petrie, W. M. F. (1899) Sequences in prehistoric remains. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute of Great Britain and Ireland 29:295301. [aAM]Google Scholar
Petroski, H. (1994) The evolution of useful things. Vintage. [arAM]Google Scholar
Philippe, H. & Douady, C. J. (2003) Horizontal gene transfer and phylogenetics. Current Opinion in Microbiology 6:498505. [rAM]Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1995) Language acquisition. In: Language: An invitation to cognitive science, vol. 1, 2nd edition, ed. Gleitman, L. R. & Liberman, M., pp. 135–82. MIT Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997) How the mind works. W. W. Norton. [aAM]Google Scholar
Pitt-Rivers, L.-G. A. L. (1875) On the evolution of culture. Journal of the Anthropological Institute 4:293308. [aAM]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. (1995) Darwin machines and the nature of knowledge. Penguin. [rAM]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. (1997) Evolution in mind: An introduction to evolutionary psychology. Harvard University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. (1999) Some psychological mechanisms of culture. In: The nature of concepts: Evolution, structure and representation, ed. Van Loocke, P., pp. 115–27. Taylor & Francis/Routledge. [rAM]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. (2000) Culture and psychological mechanisms. In: Darwinizing culture: The status of memetics as a science, ed. Aunger, R., pp. 6982. Oxford University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. (2002a) Learning from culture. In: The evolution of cultural entities, ed. Wheeler, M., Ziman, J. & Boden, M.. Oxford University Press. [BB]Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. C. (2002b) The imagined world made real. Penguin. [arAM]Google Scholar
Popper, K. R. (1979) Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Clarendon Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Portin, P. (1993) The concept of the gene – short history and present status. Quarterly Review of Biology 68:173223. [aAM]Google Scholar
Portin, P. (2002) Historical development of the concept of the gene. Journal of Medicine and Philosophy 27:257–86. [aAM]Google Scholar
Price, G. R. (1995) The nature of selection. Journal of Theoretical Biology 175: 389–96. [TK]Google Scholar
Provine, W. (1986) Sewall Wright and evolutionary biology. University of Chicago Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Prusiner, S. B. (1995) The prion diseases. Scientific American 272:4857. [RA]Google Scholar
Quiroga, R. Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C. & Fried, I. (2005) Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature 435: 1102–07. [aAM]Google Scholar
Radman, M., Matic, I. & Taddai, F. (1999) Evolution of evolvability. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 870:146–55. [SMR]Google Scholar
Raff, R. (1996) The shape of life: Genes, development, and the evolution of animal form. University of Chicago Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Ragan, M. A. (2001) Detection of lateral gene transfer among microbial genomes. Current Opinion in Genetics and Development 11:620–26. [rAM]Google Scholar
Rasmussen, N. (1987) A new model of developmental constraints as applied to the Drosophila system. Journal of Theoretical Biology 127:271301. [WCW]Google Scholar
Read, D. (1984) An algebraic account of the American kinship terminology. Current Anthropology 25:417–40. [DWR]Google Scholar
Read, D. (2001) What is kinship? In: The cultural analysis of kinship, ed. Feinberg, R. & Ottenheimer, M.. University of Illinois Press. [DWR]Google Scholar
Read, D. (2003) From behavior to culture: An assessment of cultural evolution and a new synthesis. Complexity 8(6):1741. [DWR]Google Scholar
Read, D. (2005) Kinship algebra expert systems. Social Science Computer Review 24(1):4367. [DWR]Google Scholar
Reader, S. M. (2004) Distinguishing social and asocial learning using diffusion dynamics. Learning and Behavior 32:90104. [aAM]Google Scholar
Relethford, J. H. (1998) Genetics of modern human origins and diversity. Annual Review of Anthropology 27:123. [MBM]Google Scholar
Rheindt, F. E., Grafe, T. U. & Abouheif, E. (2004) Rapidly evolving traits and the comparative method: How important is testing for phylogenetic signal Evolutionary Ecology Research 6:377–96. [MBM]Google Scholar
Rice, T. W. & Arnett, M. (2001) Civic culture and socioeconomic development in the United States: A view from the states, 1880s–1990s. Social Science Journal 38:3951. [rAM]Google Scholar
Rice, T. W. & Feldman, J. L. (1997) Civic culture and democracy from Europe to America. Journal of Politics 59:1143–72. [rAM]Google Scholar
Richards, R. (1992) The meaning of evolution. University of Chicago Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J. & Boyd, R. (2005) Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press. [RA, SB, AF, TK, RLL, arAM, WM, BS, WCW]Google Scholar
Ridley, M. (2003) Nature via nurture. Harper Collins. [rAM]Google Scholar
Riede, F. (in press) Maglemosian memes: Technological ontology, craft traditions and the evolution of Northern European barbed points. In: Cultural transmission and archaeology: Issues and case-studies, ed. O’Brien, M. J.. Society for American Archaeology. [rAM]Google Scholar
Rindos, D. (1985) Darwinian selection, symbolic variation, and the evolution of culture. Current Anthropology 26:6588. [rAM]Google Scholar
Rindos, D. (1989) Undirected variation and the Darwinian explanation of cultural change. Archaeological Method and Theory 1:145. [MJO]Google Scholar
Rivera, M. C. & Lake, J. A. (2004) The ring of life provides evidence for a genome fusion origin of eukaryotes. Nature 431:152–55. [arAM]Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. (1996) Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. Brain Research 3:131–41. [aAM]Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L. & Gallese, V. (2002) From mirror neurons to imitation: Facts and speculations. In: The imitative mind: Development, evolution and brain bases, ed. Meltzoff, A. N. & Prinz, W., pp. 247266. Cambridge University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Roca, A. L., Bar-Gal, G. K., Eizirik, E., Helgen, K. M., Maria, R., Springer, M. S., O’Brien, S. J. & Murphy, W. J. (2004) Mesozoic origin for West Indian insectivores. Nature 429:649–51. [aAM]Google Scholar
Rogers, E. (1995) The diffusion of innovations. Free Press. [arAM]Google Scholar
Rosnow, R. L. (1980) Psychology of rumor reconsidered. Psychological Bulletin 87:578–91. [aAM]Google Scholar
Rosnow, R. L. (1991) Inside rumor: A personal journey. American Psychologist 46:484– 96. [aAM]Google Scholar
Rumiati, R. I., Weiss, P. H., Shallice, T., Ottoboni, G., Noth, J., Zilles, K. & Fink, G. R. (2004) Neural basis of pantomiming the use of visually presented objects. Neuroimage 21:1224–31. [aAM]Google Scholar
Runciman, W. G. (2005) Culture does evolve. History and Theory 44:113. [aAM]Google Scholar
Ryan, B. & Gross, N. (1943) The diffusion of hybrid seed corn in two Iowa communities. Rural Sociology 8:1524. [aAM]Google Scholar
Sahlins, M. & Service, E. (1960) Evolution and culture. Ann Arbor. [rAM]Google Scholar
Salmon, C. (1998) The evocative nature of kin terminology in political rhetoric. Politics and the Life Sciences 17:5157. [LC]Google Scholar
Sanz, C., Morgan, D. & Gulick, S. (2004) New insights into chimpanzees, tools and termites from the Congo basin. American Naturalist 164:567–81. [rAM]Google Scholar
Schank, J. & Wimsatt, W. (1988) Generative entrenchment and evolution. In: PSA- 1986, vol. II, ed. Fine, A. & Machamer, P. K.. Philosophy of Science Association. [WCW]Google Scholar
Schank, J. & Wimsatt, W. (2000) Evolvability: Modularity and generative entrenchment. In: Thinking about evolution: Historical, philosophical and political perspectives. Festschrift for Richard Lewontin, vol. 2, ed. Singh, R., Krimbas, C., Paul, D., & Beatty, J.. Cambridge University Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Schleicher, A. (1863) Darwinism tested by the science of language, trans. Bikkers, A. V. M.. Böhlau. Reprinted in: (1983) Linguistics and evolutionary theory: Three essays, ed. Koerner, K.. John Benjamins. [LC]Google Scholar
Schlosser, G. & Wagner, G. (2004) Modularity in evolution and development. University of Chicago Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Schmitt-Ott, U. & Wimmer, E. (2004) Starting the segmentation gene cascade in insects. In: Modularity in evolution and development, ed. Schlosser, G. & Wagner, G.. University of Chicago Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Schotter, A. (1981) The economic theory of social institutions. Cambridge University Press. [BS]Google Scholar
Schotter, A. & Sopher, B. (2003) Social learning and coordination conventions in intergenerational games: An experimental study. Journal of Political Economy 111(3):498529. [BS, aAM]Google Scholar
Schotter, A. & Sopher, B. (2006) Advice, trust and trustworthiness in an experimental intergenerational game. Experimental Economics 9(2):123–45. [BS]Google Scholar
Schotter, A. & Sopher, B. (2006) (forthcoming) Advice and behavior in intergenerational ultimatum games: An experimental approach. Games and Economic Behavior. [BS]Google Scholar
Schumpeter, J. F. (1934) The theory of economic development: An inquiry into profits, capital, credit, interest, and the business cycle, trans. Opie, R.. Harvard University Press. [BS]Google Scholar
Schwartz, T. & Mead, M. (1961) Micro- and macro-cultural models for cultural evolution. Anthropological Linguistics 3:17. [rAM]Google Scholar
Seiffert, E. R., Simons, E. L. & Attia, Y. (2003) Fossil evidence for an ancient divergence of lorises and galagos. Nature 422:421–24. [aAM]Google Scholar
Shennan, S. (2002) Genes, memes and human history. Thames and Hudson. [aAM, JT]Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S. J. (2000) Modularity and the evolution of cognition. In: The evolution of cognition, ed. Heyes, C. M. & Huber, L., pp. 43–60. MIT Press. [SMR]Google Scholar
Simons, A. M. (2002) The continuity of microevolution and macroevolution. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 15:688701. [DWR]Google Scholar
Simonton, D. K. (1995) Foresight in insight? A Darwinian answer. In: The nature of insight, ed. Sternberg, R. J., pp. 465–94. MIT Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Simpson, G. G. (1944) Tempo and mode in evolution. Columbia University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Simpson, G. G. (1961) Principles of animal taxonomy. Columbia University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1981) Selection by consequences. Science 213:501504. [aAM]Google Scholar
Smith, E. A. & Winterhalder, B., eds. (1992) Evolutionary ecology and human behavior. Aldine de Gruyter. [aAM]Google Scholar
Sober, E. (1992) Models of cultural evolution. In: Trees of life: Essays in philosophy of biology, ed. Griffiths, P.. Kluwer. [MJO]Google Scholar
Sober, E. & Wilson, D. S. (1998) Unto others: The evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. Harvard University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Sosis, R. (2000) Costly signaling and torch fishing on Ifaluk Atoll. Evolution and Human Behavior 21:223–44. [LC]Google Scholar
Sosis, R. & Alcorta, C. (2003) Signaling, solidarity, and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology 12:264–74. [LC]Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1996) Explaining culture. Blackwell. [WCW]Google Scholar
(2002) In defense of massive modularity. In: Language, brain and cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler, ed. Dupoux, E.. MIT Press. [SMR]Google Scholar
Sperber, D. & Hirschfeld, L. A. (2004) The cognitive foundations of cultural stability and diversity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8:4046. [aAM, SMR]Google Scholar
Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1986) Relevance: Communication and cognition. Blackwell. [rAM]Google Scholar
Spier, L. (1935) The Prophet Dance of the Northwest and its derivatives: The source of the Ghost Dance. American Anthropological Association General Series in Anthropology, Monograph No. 1, pp.1–74. [RLL]Google Scholar
Stanley, S. (1975) A theory of evolution above the species level. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 72:646–50. [rAM]Google Scholar
Stephens, D. W. & Krebs, J. R. (1986) Foraging theory. Princeton University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Stephens, P. R. & Wiens, J. J. (2004) Convergence, divergence, and homogenization in the ecological structure of emydid turtle communities: The effects of phylogeny and dispersal. American Naturalist 164:244–54. [aAM]Google Scholar
Steward, J. (1955) Theory of culture change. University of Illinois Press. [arAM]Google Scholar
Stotz, K. & Griffiths, P. (2004) Genes: Philosophical analyses put to the test. History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 26:528. [aAM]Google Scholar
Swofford, D. L. (1998) PAUP*4: Phylogenetic analyses using parsimony (*and other methods) version 4. Sinauer. [aAM]Google Scholar
Symonds, M. R. E. (2002) The effects of topological inaccuracy in evolutionary trees on the phylogenetic comparative method of independent contrasts. Systematic Biology 51(4):541–53. [MBM]Google Scholar
Tehrani, J. J. & Collard, M. (2002) Investigating cultural evolution through biological phylogenetic analyses of Turkmen textiles. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 21:443–63. [arAM, JT]Google Scholar
Terkel, J. (1996) Cultural transmission of feeding behaviour in the black rat (Rattus rattus) In: Social learning in animals: The roots of culture, ed. Heyes, C. M. & Galef, B. G., pp. 1748. Academic Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Thomason, S. G. & Kaufman, T. (1988) Language contact, creolization, and genetic linguistics. University of California Press. [MBM]Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press. [aAM, TK]Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. & Moll, H. (2005) Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28(5):675–91. [rAM]Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C. & Ratner, H. H. (1993) Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16(3):495511. [RA, arAM]Google Scholar
Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (1992) The psychological foundations of culture. In: The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J., pp. 19136. Oxford University Press. [RA, arAM]Google Scholar
Torres-Vila, L. M., Gragera, J., Rodriguez-Molina, M. C. & Stockel, J. (2002) Heritable variation for female remating in Lobesia botrana, a usually monandrous moth. Animal Behaviour 64:899907. [aAM]Google Scholar
Turner, M. (1991) Reading minds: The study of English in the age of cognitive science. Princeton University Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1871) Primitive culture. Murray. [arAM, MJO]Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singleton, I., Suzuki, A., Utami, S. S. & Merrill, M. (2003) Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science 299:102105. [arAM]Google Scholar
Vermeij, G. J. (1996) Adaptations of clades: Resistance and response. In: Adaptation, ed. Rose, M. R. & Lauder, G.. Academic. [rAM]Google Scholar
Wade, M. & Goodnight, C. (1998) The theories of Fisher andWright in the context of meta-populations: When nature does many small experiments. Evolution 52:1537–53. [WCW]Google Scholar
Wagner, G. ed. (2000) The character concept in evolutionary biology. Academic Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Wagner, G. P. & Altenberg, L. (1996) Perspective: Complex adaptations and the evolution of evolvability. Evolution 50:967–76. [SMR]Google Scholar
Walsh, A. (1997) Methodological individualism and vertical integration in the social sciences. Behavior and Philosophy 25(2):121–36. [JHB]Google Scholar
Want, S. C. & Harris, P. L. (2002) How do children ape? Applying concepts from the study of non-human primates to the developmental study of “imitation” in children. Developmental Science 5:113. [aAM]Google Scholar
Warner, R. R. (1990) Resource assessment versus tradition in mating-site determination. American Naturalist 135:205–17. [arAM]Google Scholar
Watson, J. D., Hopkins, N. H., Roberts, J. W. & Weiner, A. M. (1987) Molecular biology of the gene. Benjamin/Cummings. [aAM]Google Scholar
Weber, R. & Camerer, C. (2003) Cultural conflict and merger failure: An experimental approach. Management Science 49(4):400–15. [BS]Google Scholar
Weber, R. J., Dixon, S. & Llorente, A. M. (1993) Studying invention: The hand tool as a model system. Science, Technology and Human Values 18:480505. [rAM]Google Scholar
Weber, R. J. & Perkins, D. N. (1989) How to invent artifacts and ideas. New Ideas in Psychology 7:4972. [rAM]Google Scholar
Weill, J.-C. & Reynaud, C.-A. (1996) Rearrangement/hypermutation/gene conversion: When, where and why? Immunology Today 17:9297. [SMR]Google Scholar
Wermke, K. & Mende, W. (2006) Melody as a primordial legacy from early roots of language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29(3):300. Commentary on Locke, J. L. & Bogin, B. Language and life history: A new perspective on the development and evolution of human language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29(3):259–80. [WM]Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003) Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press. [SMR]Google Scholar
Wheeler, M., Ziman, J. & Boden, M. A., eds. (2002) The evolution of cultural entities. Oxford University Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
White, L. (1959) The evolution of culture. McGraw-Hill. [arAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (1998) Imitation of the sequential structure of actions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology 112:270–81. [rAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2005a) The imitative correspondence problem: Solved or sidestepped? In: Perspectives on imitation, ed. Hurley, S. & Chater, N., pp. 220222. MIT Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2005b) The second inheritance system of chimpanzees and humans. Nature 437:5255. [arAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Custance, D. M., Gomez, J. C., Teixidor, P. & Bard, K. A. (1996) Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology 110:314. [arAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Flynn, E., Brown, K. & Lee, T. (in press) Imitation of hierarchical action structure by young children. Developmental Science. [rAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W. & Boesch, C. (1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature 399:682–85. [arAM, SMR]Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Goodall, J., McGrew, W. C., Nishida, T., Reynolds, V., Sugiyama, Y., Tutin, C. E. G., Wrangham, R. W. & Boesch, C. (2001) Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour 138:1481–516. [aAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A. & Ham, R. (1992) On the nature and evolution of imitation in the animal kingdom: Reappraisal of a century of research. In: Advances in the study of behaviour, ed. Slater, P. J. B., Rosenblatt, J. S., Beer, C. & Milinski, M., pp. 239283. Academic Press. [aAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V. & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005) Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature 437:737–40. [arAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004) How do apes ape? Learning and Behavior 32:3652. [aAM]Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V. & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2003) Cultural panthropology. Evolutionary Anthropology 12:92105. [aAM]Google Scholar
Whiting, M. F., Bradler, S. & Maxwell, T. (2003) Loss and recovery of wings in stick insects. Nature 421:264–67. [aAM]Google Scholar
Wiens, J. J. (2004) What is speciation and how should we study it? American Naturalist 163:914–23. [aAM]Google Scholar
Williams, G. (1966) Adaptation and natural selection. Princeton University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Williams, J. H. G., Waiter, G. D., Gilchrist, A., Perrett, D. I., Murray, A. D. & Whiten, A. (2006) Neural mechanisms of imitation and “mirror neuron” functioning in autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia 44:610–21. [rAM]Google Scholar
Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T. & Perrett, D. I. (2001) Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 25:287–95. [aAM]Google Scholar
Williamson, O. E. (2000) The new institutional economics: Taking stock, looking ahead. Journal of Economic Literature 38:595613. [BS]Google Scholar
Wilson, C. B. & Yost, J. (2001) The cultural construction of hierarchy. In: Ethnographic essays in cultural anthropology, ed. Morrison, R. B. & Wilson, C. B., pp. 101–25. F.E. Peacock. [DWR]Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S. & Sober, E. (1994) Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17(4):585608. [rAM]Google Scholar
Wimsatt, W. (1986) Developmental constraints, generative entrenchment, and the innate-acquired distinction. In: Integrating scientific disciplines, ed. Bechtel, W.. Martinus-Nijhoff. [WCW]Google Scholar
Wimsatt, W. (1999) Genes, memes, and cultural inheritance. Biology and Philosophy 14:279310. [WCW]Google Scholar
Wimsatt, W. (2001) Generative entrenchment and the developmental systems approach to evolutionary processes. In: Cycles of contingency: Developmental systems and evolution, ed. Oyama, S., Gray, R. & Griffiths, P.. MIT Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Wimsatt, W. (2003) Evolution, entrenchment, and innateness. In: Reductionism and the growth of knowledge, ed. Brown, T. & Smith, L.. Erlbaum. [WCW]Google Scholar
Wimsatt, W. & Griesemer, J. (2006) Re-producing entrenchments to scaffold culture: The central role of development in cultural evolution. In: Integrating evolution and development, ed. Sansom, R. & Brandon, R.. MIT Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Wimsatt, W. & Schank, J. (1988) Two constraints on the evolution of complex adaptations and the means for their avoidance. In: Evolutionary progress, ed. Nitecki, M.. University of Chicago Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Wimsatt, W. & Schank, J. (2004) Generative entrenchment, modularity and evolvability: When genic selection meets the whole organism. In: Modularity in evolution and development, ed. Schlosser, G. & Wagner, G.. University of Chicago Press. [WCW]Google Scholar
Wolf, J. B., Brodie, E. D. I. & Wade, M. J. (2000) Epistasis and the evolutionary process. Oxford University Press. [rAM]Google Scholar
Wood, D., Bruner, J. S. & Ross, G. (1976) The role of tutoring in problem-solving. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 17:89100. [rAM]Google Scholar
Wright, S. (1931) Evolution in Mendelian populations. Genetics 16:97159. [aAM]Google Scholar
Young, H. P. (1993) The evolution of conventions. Econometrica 61:5784. [BS]Google Scholar
Young, H. P. (1998) Individual strategy and social structure: An evolutionary theory. Princeton University Press. [BS, TK]Google Scholar
Zhou, Z. H., Barrett, P. M. & Hilton, J. (2003) An exceptionally preserved lower cretaceous ecosystem. Nature 421:807–14. [aAM]Google Scholar
Ziman, J., ed. (2000) Technological innovation as an evolutionary process. Cambridge University Press. [arAM]Google Scholar