Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T15:54:49.071Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Responses to music: Emotional signaling, and learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2008

Martin F. Gardiner
Affiliation:
Center for the Study of Human Development, Brown University, Providence, RI 02828. [email protected]

Abstract

In the target article, Juslin & Västfjäll (J&V) contend that neural mechanisms not unique to music are critical to its capability to convey emotion. The work reviewed here provides a broader context for this proposal. Human abilities to signal emotion through sound could have been essential to human evolution, and may have contributed vital foundations for music. Future learning experiments are needed to further clarify engagement underlying musical and broader emotional signaling.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Cannon, W. B. (1929) Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear and rage, 2nd edition. Appleton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, W. G. & Simon, H. A. (1973) Perception in chess. Cognitive Psychology 4:5581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clynes, M. (1977) Sentics: The touch of emotions. Doubleday.Google Scholar
DeGroot, A. D. (1965) Thought and choice in chess. Moulton.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1980) Art as experience. Perigee Books.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1997) Facial action coding system. Consulting Pyschologists Press.Google Scholar
Gaab, N., Tallal, P., Kim, H., Lakshnariyann, K., Glover, G. H. & Gabrielli, J. D. E. (2005) Rapid spectrotemporal processing in musicians and nonmusicians. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1060:8288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardiner, M. F. (2000) Music, learning and behavior: A case for mental stretching. Journal for Learning Through Music 1:7293.Google Scholar
Gardiner, M. F. (2003) Music. In: Encyclopedia of human ecology, vol. 2, ed. Lerner, R. M., Schaimberg, L. B., Anderson, P. M. & Miller, J., pp. 509–14. ABC-CLIO.Google Scholar
Gardiner, M. F. (2008a) Skill learning, brain engagement, context and the arts. In: Simultaneity: Temporal structures and observer perspectives, ed. Vrobel, S., Rössler, O. E. & Marks-Tarlow, T., pp. 195214. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardiner, M. F. (2008b) Academic progress in students receiving musical skill training. In: Music instruction that tips the scale in favour of student achievement. Paper presented at the 68th Annual Conference, National School Board Association, Orlando, Florida, March 21–April 1, 2008.Google Scholar
Gardiner, M. F., Fox, A., Knowles, F. & Jeffrey, D. (1996) Learning improved by arts training. Nature 381:284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, W. & Lange, C. (1922) The emotions. Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Rauscher, F. H., Shaw, G. L., Levine, L. J., Wright, E. I., Dennis, W. R. & Newcomb, R. L. (1977) Music training causes long-term enhancement of preschool children's spatial-temporal reasoning abilities. Neurological Research 19:18.Google Scholar
Tallal, P. & Gaab, N. (2006) Dynamic auditory processing, musical experience and language development. Trends in Neurosciences 29(7):382–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed