Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dlnhk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T23:57:17.041Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Learning and memory are inextricable

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 September 2024

Sue Llewellyn*
Affiliation:
University of Manchester, Manchester, UK [email protected] https://www.humanities.manchester.ac.uk/
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

The authors' aim is to build “more biologically plausible learning algorithms” that work in naturalistic environments. Given that, first, human learning and memory are inextricable, and, second, that much human learning is unconscious, can the authors' first research question of how people improve their learning abilities over time be answered without addressing these two issues? I argue that it cannot.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Learning is the process of acquiring a memory; learning and memory both depend, fundamentally, on association (Fuster, Reference Fuster1999). The inextricability of learning and memory originates because any to-be-learned information is encoded in memory through association, the same associative encoding also enables retrieval from memory (Brown & Craik, Reference Brown, Craik, Tulving and Craik2000; Tulving & Thomson, Reference Tulving and Thomson1973). Once retrieved, the encoded memory will drive expectations in the same (or similar) environments to those in which the learning took place.

The authors identify their meta-learned models as ones that “acquire their inductive biases from experience, i.e. by repeatedly interacting with an environment.” This implies that learning is cumulative over time as the model repeatedly samples the environment. For human learning to be cumulative across time, the learned information must be encoded and retained in memory. Later in the paper, the authors, briefly, acknowledge the contribution of episodic memory and the hippocampus but do not spell out how hippocampal memory systems impact their models.

In relation to the authors' first research question, people can improve their learning abilities through elaborate encoding which creates associations between the to-be-learnt episodic material and information already encoded in episodic memory networks (Foer, Reference Foer2011; Llewellyn, Reference Llewellyn2013; Yates, Reference Yates1966). The hippocampus is crucial to both associative encoding and later retrieval of episodes through association (Davachi & Wagner, Reference Davachi and Wagner2002; Llewellyn, Reference Llewellyn2013; Thakral, Benoit, & Schacter, Reference Thakral, Benoit and Schacter2017). The sequential, associative nature of elements of learned, encoded and retained episodic memories gives rise to rational, step-by-step learning. However, during rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, the hippocampus may take elements of different episodic experiences to identify more elaborative, associative, probabilistic and meaningful patterns in experience which may be expressed, visually, as dreams and instantiated as cortical nodes/junctions during non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep (Llewellyn & Hobson, Reference Llewellyn and Hobson2015). Cortical episodic memory networks then consist of sequential, associative pathways which converge at nodes/junctions which express patterned, probabilistic, experiential learning. The hidden units/nodes in neural networks may be analogous to these nodes/junctions in cortical episodic memory networks.

Given the authors' recognition, “that episodic memory could be the key to explaining human performance in naturalistic environments” it seems pertinent to expand their algorithms to encompass not only the logical, experiential, “step-by-step” learning patterns that predominate during wake but also the more complex, hyper-associative, experiential patterns identified during REM sleep. Admittedly, this extension may be, somewhat, futuristic. But research has already mimicked slow wave sleep which restored stability to neural networks engaged in unsupervised learning (Watkins, Kim, Sornborger, & Kenyon, Reference Watkins, Kim, Sornborger and Kenyon2020). Also, given the adaptivity, speed via massive parallelism, fault tolerance and optimality of neural networks (Bezdek, Reference Bezdek1992) and that machine learning programmes trained on probabilistic reasoning are superior to the human brain for visual pattern recognition (Pavlus, Reference Pavlus2016) it may be possible that, in the recurrent neural networks mobilized by the authors, REM sleep is not required for complex pattern recognition.

Experiences in natural environments may never be re-enacted in their entirety but they are certainly non-random and do not exclude expectations. Across evolutionary time, many interactions with the environment held dangers because of predators and competitors. To try to avoid dangers, humans needed to identify the probabilistic, behavioural patterns of predators and competitors. Such patterns would only be revealed over several episodes, as humans observed the associations that drove predator behaviour. For example, lions tend to visit waterholes at night when prey are abundant (lion presence is associated with nighttime) but, in the dry season, lions get so thirsty that they may be at the waterhole during the day (in the dry season, lion presence can be associated with daytime). Also, elephants tend to drive lions away from the waterhole, so the presence of elephants offers some safety (elephant presence is associated with lion absence). When many associations are at stake and/or actions must be fast, probabilistic associative patterns, derived from multiple episodes, drive expectations and learning during future interactions in the environment.

Much, probably the majority, of learning occurs unconsciously. Contemporarily, dangers may differ but unconscious expectations, formed through learning and retained as unconscious memories, still drive fast responses to threats (Öhman, Carlsson, Lundqvist, & Ingvar, Reference Öhman, Carlsson, Lundqvist and Ingvar2007). Concomitantly, it would be expected that the elaborative, complex, associative, probabilistic and meaningful experiential patterns formed in REM sleep would be unconscious. Arzi et al. (Reference Arzi, Shedlesky, Ben-Shaul, Nasser, Oksenberg, Hairston and Sobel2012) showed that new associations, learned during REM sleep, were retained as unconscious memories, then functioning, during wake, as unconscious expectations which drove actions.

Does the human unconscious have any parallels in machine learning? We know that machine learning algorithms inherit unconscious human biases, indeed they can amplify them (Thomas, Reference Thomas2018). Moreover, humans can continue to reproduce machine learning bias, even after they no longer use the algorithm (Vicente & Matute, Reference Vicente and Matute2023). Clearly, unconscious learning and memory are not subject to voluntary control. Although unconscious biases can be detrimental (e.g., to marginalized groups) inductive, unconscious (or implicit) biases are essential for faster information processing both in humans and machine learning. Inductive biases depend on the associations formed through experience, in machine learning such associations occur during unsupervised learning. Specifically, in the authors' meta-learned models, associations will be experiential.

In sum, the authors assert that their meta-learned models are “invaluable tools to study, analyse and understand the human mind.” In humans, experiential, associative learning becomes associative memories that, then, drive expectations in subsequent learning. In the memory network architecture of the brain/mind these associative memories may take the form of the serial, associative pathways which underlie conscious learning. Equally, where these serial pathways cross at nodes/junctions, complex, hyper-associative, experiential patterns arise from elements of the different intersecting pathways. These patterns are retained as unconscious memories which associate elements of different experiences.

Association is fundamental to both learning and memory, whether conscious or unconscious. The authors' first research question of how people improve their learning abilities over time can be better addressed through acknowledging, first, the inextricability of learning and memory and, second, the role of conscious and unconscious association in each.

Financial support

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Competing interests

None.

References

Arzi, A., Shedlesky, L., Ben-Shaul, M., Nasser, K., Oksenberg, A., Hairston, I. S., & Sobel, N. (2012). Humans can learn new information during sleep. Nature neuroscience, 15(10), 14601465. doi: 10.1038/nn.3193CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bezdek, J. C. (1992). On the relationship between neural networks, pattern recognition and intelligence. International Journal of Approximate Reasoning, 6(2), 85107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, S. C., & Craik, F. I. M. (2000). Encoding and retrieval of information. In Tulving, E. & Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 93107). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davachi, L., & Wagner, A. D. (2002). Hippocampal contributions to episodic encoding: Insights from relational and item-based learning. Journal of Neurophysiology, 88(2), 982990. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.982CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foer, J. (2011). Moonwalking with Einstein: The art and science of remembering everything. Allen Lane.Google Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (1999). Memory in the cerebral cortex: An empirical approach to neural networks in the human and nonhuman primate. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Llewellyn, S. (2013). Such stuff as dreams are made on? Elaborative encoding, the ancient art of memory, and the hippocampus. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(6), 589607. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12003135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Llewellyn, S., & Hobson, J. A. (2015). Not only… but also: REM sleep creates and NREM stage 2 instantiates landmark junctions in cortical memory networks. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 122, 6987. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2015.04.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Öhman, A., Carlsson, K., Lundqvist, D., & Ingvar, M. (2007). On the unconscious subcortical origin of human fear. Physiology & behavior, 92(1–2), 180185. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlus, J. (2016). Computers now recognize patterns better than humans can. Scientific American. Originally published as, “8. Sight-Reading Software” in Scientific American Magazine Vol. 315 No. 6 (December 2016), p. 41. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican1216-39bCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thakral, P. P., Benoit, R. G., & Schacter, D. L. (2017). Characterizing the role of the hippocampus during episodic simulation and encoding. Hippocampus, 27(12), 12751284. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22796CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, R. (2018). Analyzing & preventing unconscious bias in machine learning. Retrieved from: QCon.ai 2018.Google Scholar
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vicente, L., & Matute, H. (2023). Humans inherit artificial intelligence biases. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 15737. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-42384-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, Y., Kim, E., Sornborger, A., & Kenyon, G. T. (2020). Using sinusoidally-modulated noise as a surrogate for slow-wave sleep to accomplish stable unsupervised dictionary learning in a spike-based sparse coding model. In Proceedings of the IEEE/CVF Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition Workshops (pp. 360361).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yates, F. A. (1966). The art of memory. Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar