No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
Waiting for the world to make me talk and tell me what I meant
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1984
References
Anderson, J. R. (1978) Arguments concerning representations for mental imagery. Psychological Review 85:249–77. [LCR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, D. M. (1971) Meaning and communication Philosophical Review 80:427–47. [JB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. (1976) Language and context: The acquisition of pragmatics. Academic Press. [RPB]Google Scholar
Bem, D. J. (1967) Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review 74:183–200. [DTK, rBFS, HST]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bem, D. J. & Allen, A. (1974) On predicting some of the people some of the time: The search for cross-situational consistencies in behavior. Psychological Review 81:506183–20. [DTK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennett, J. (1975) Stimulus, response, meaning. American Philosophical Quarterly 9:55–88. [JB]Google Scholar
Bergmann, G. & Spence, K. W. (1941) Operationism and theory in psychology. Psychological Review 48:1–14. [JM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, N. (1978) Troubles with functionalism. In: Perception and cognition: Issues in the foundations of psychology. Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science, vol. 9, ed. Savage, C. W.. University of Minnesota Press. [GG]Google Scholar
Boden, M. (1972) Purposive explanation in psychology. Harvard University Press. [JDR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1936) Temporal perception and operationism. American Journal of Psychology 48:519–22. [JM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1945) The use of operational definitions in science. Psychological Review 52:243–45. [taBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bousfield, A. K. & Bousfield, W. A. (1966) Measurement of clustering and of sequential constancies in repeated free recall. Psychological Reports 19:935–42. [HST]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, G. H. (1972) A selective review of organizational factors in memory. In: Organization of memory, ed. Tulving, E. & Donaldson, W.. Academic Press. [HST]Google Scholar
Bridgman, P. W. (1945) Some general principles of operational analysis. Psychological Review 52:246–49. [taBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinker, R. P. (1982) Contextual contours in the development of language. In: Children thinking through language, ed. Beveridge, M.. Edward Arnold. [RPB]Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1975) The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language 2:1–19. [RPB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cheek, J. M. (1982) Aggregation, moderator variables, and the validity of personality tests: A peer-rating study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43:1254–69. [DTK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959) A review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Rehavior. Language 35:26–58. [RPB, JDR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P. (1981) Eliminative materialism and propositional attitudes. Journal of Philosophy 78:67–90. [JB]Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. & Churchland, P. S. (1981) Functionalism, qualia, and intentionality. Philosophical Topics 12:121–45. [GG)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Commons, M. L., Hermstein, R. J. & Rachlin, H. eds. (1982) Quantitative analyses of behavior, vol. 2, Matching and maximizing accounts. Ballinger, [HR]Google Scholar
Cumming, W. W. & Berryman, R. R. (1965) Stimulus generalization. In: Stimulus generalization, ed. Mostofsky, D. I.. Stanford University Press. [HST]Google Scholar
Feigl, H. (1945) Operationism and scientific method. Psychological Review 52:250–59. [RPB, taBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1980) Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:63–72. [JB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A., Bever, T. G. & Garrett, M. F. (1974) The psychology of language. McGraw-Hill. [JDR]Google Scholar
Grant, D. S. (1983) Rehearsal in pigion short-term memory. In: Animal cognition, ed. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. & Terrace, H. S.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [HST]Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. & Smith, J. H. (1976) The structure of communication in early language development. Academic Press. [RPB]Google Scholar
Harré, R. & Secord, P. F. (1972) The explanation of social behaviour. Blackwell. [CFL]Google Scholar
Hempel, C. G. (1965a) The concept of rationality and the logic of explanations by reasons. In: Aspects of scientific explanations. Free Press. [JDR]Google Scholar
Hempel, C. G. (1965b) Empiricist criteria of cognitive significance: Problems and changes. In: Aspects of scientific explanation. Free Press. [JDR]Google Scholar
Hempel, C. G. (1965c) A logical appraisal of operationism. In: Aspects of scientific explanation. Free Press. [JDR]Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1977) The evolution of behaviorism. American Psychologist 32:593–603. [RPB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinsie, L. E. & Campbell, R. J., eds. (1970) Psychiatric Dictionary. 4th ed.Oxford University Press. [PEM]Google Scholar
Jaynes, J. (1976) The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. Houghton Mifflin. [HST]Google Scholar
Kendall, P. C. & Hollon, S. D. (1979) Cognitive-behavioral interventions: Theory, research and procedures. Academic Press. (CFL]Google Scholar
Kenrick, D. T. & Dantchik, A. (1983) Interactions, idiographics, and the social psychological invasion of personality. Journal of Personality 51:286–307. [DTK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenrick, D. T. & Stringfield, D. O. (1980) Personality traits and the eye of the beholder: Crossing some traditional philosophical boundaries in the search for consistency in all of the people. Psychological Review 87:88–104. [DTK]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, S. (1964) Psychology and emerging conceptions of knowledge as unitary. In: Behaviorism and phenomenology, ed. Wann, T. W.. University of Chicago Press. [JDR]Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1962) The structure of scientific revolutions. University of Chicago Press. [JDR]Google Scholar
Lacey, H. (1974) The scientific study of linguistic behavior: A perspective on the Skinner-Chomsky controversy. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 4:17–51. [JDR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ledgwidge, B. (1978) Cognitive-behavior modification: A step in the wrong direction? Psychological Bulletin 85:353–75. [CFL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Locke, E. A. (1979) Behavior modification is not cognitive and other myths: A reply to Ledgwidge. Cognitive Therapy and Research 3:119–25. [CFL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lowe, C. F. (1979) Determinants of human operant behaviour. In: Advances in analysis of behaviour, vol. 1, Reinforcement and the organisation of behaviour, ed. Zeiler, M. D. & Harzem, P.. Wiley. [CFL]Google Scholar
Lowe, C. F. (1983) Radical behaviorism and human psychology. In: Animal models of human behavior, ed. Davey, G. C. L.. Wiley. [CFL, rBFS]Google Scholar
Lowe, C. F., Beasty, A. & Bentall, R. P. (1983) The role of verbal behavior in human learning: Infant performance on fixed-interval schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 39:157–64. [CFL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, C. F. & Higson, P. J. (1981) Self-instructional training and cognitive behaviour modification: A behavioural analysis. In: Applications of conditioning theory, ed. Davey, G. C. L.. Methuen. [CFL]Google Scholar
Luria, A. (1961) The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. Liveright. [CFL]Google Scholar
Lycan, W. (1981) Form, function, and feel. Journal of Philosophy 78:24–49. [GG]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacCorquodale, K. (1970) On Chomsky's review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 13:83–99. [JDR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mach, E. (1919) The science of mechanics. Translated by McCormack, T. J.. 4th ed.Open Court Publishing Company. [JDR]Google Scholar
Mahoney, M. J. (1977) Reflections on the cognitive-learning trend in psychotherapy. American Psychologist 32:5–13. [CFL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malcolm, N. (1964) Behaviorism as a philosophy of psychology. In: Behaviorism and phenomenology: Contrasting bases for modern psychology, ed. Wann, T. W.. University of Chicago Press. [GG, rBFS]Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1967) Organization and memory. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, ed. Spence, K. W. & Spence, J. T.. Academic Press. [HST]Google Scholar
Olton, D. S. & Samuelson, R. J. (1976) Remembrance of places past: Spatial memory in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 2:97–116. [HST]Google Scholar
Palmer, S. E. (1978) Fundamental aspects of cognitive representation. In: Cognition and categorization, ed. Rosch, E. & Lloyd, B.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [LCR]Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1969) The gaps in empiricism. In: Beyond reductionism, ed. Koestler, A. & Smythies, J. R.. Hutchinson. [RPB]Google Scholar
Place, U. T. (1981a) Skinner's Verbal behavior I – Why we need it. Behaviorism 9:1–24. [UTP]Google Scholar
Place, U. T. (1981b) Skinner's Verbal Behavior II – What is wrong with it. Behaviorism 9:131–52. [UTP]Google Scholar
Quine, W. (1970) Methodological reflections on current linguistic theory. Synthese 27:325–29. [JDR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ringen, J. (1977) On evaluating data concerning linguistic intuition. In: Current themes in linguistics, ed. Eckman, F.. Wiley. [JDR]Google Scholar
Ringen, J. (1980) Quine on introspection in linguistics. In: A festschrift for a native speaker, ed. Coulmas, F.. Mouton. [JDR]Google Scholar
Roberts, W. A. & Kraemer, P. J. (1982) Some observations of the effects of intertrial interval and delay on delayed matching to sample in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 8:342–53. [HST]Google ScholarPubMed
Roitblat, H. L. (1980) Codes and coding processes in pigeon short-term memory. Animal Learning and Behavior 8:341–51. [HST]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scriven, M. (1956) A study of radical behaviorism. In: The foundations of science and the concepts of psychology and psychoanalysis, ed. Feigl, H. & Scriven, M.. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, vol. 1. University of Minnesota Press. [JDR]Google Scholar
Segal, E. F. (1975) Psycholingiiistics discovers the operant: A review of Roger Brown's A first language: The early states. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 23:149–58. [RPB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N. (1975) Form, formation and transformation of internal representations. In: Information processing and cognition, ed. Solso, R. L.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [HST]Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N. & Metzler, J. (1971) Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171:701–3. [LCR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shimp, C. P. (1976) Short-term memory in the pigeon: Relative recency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 25:55–61. [HST]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1931) The concept of the reflex in the description of behavior. Journal of General Psychology 5:427–58. [PNH, JDR, taBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1935a) The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. Journal of General Psychology 12:40–65. [RPB, PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1935b) Two types of conditioned reflex and a pseudo-type. Journal of General Psychology 12:66–77. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1937) Two types of conditioned reflex: A reply to Konorski and Miller. Journal of General Psychology 16:272–79. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938) The behavior of organisms. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [RPB, CFL, PEM, DS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1950) Are theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review 57:193–216. [PNH, rBFS, HST]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1957) Verbal behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [KRG, GC, CFL, JM, UTP, JDR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1959) John Broadus Watson, behaviorist. Science 129:197–98. [JDR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1969a) Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [RPB, UTP, HST, GEZ]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1969b) An operant analysis of problem solving. In: Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [PNH]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1977a) Herrnstein and the evolution of behaviorism. American Psychologist 32:1006–12. [RPB, PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1977b) Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism 5:1–10 [HST]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1981) Selection by consequences. Science 213:501–4. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slater, E. & Glithero, E. (1965) A follow up of patients diagnosed as suffering from hysteria. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 9:9–13. [DTK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, S. S. (1939) Psychology and the science of science. Psychological Bulletin 36:221–63. [JM, taBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrace, H. S. (1970) Towards a doctrine of radical behaviorism. Contemporary Psychology 15:531–35. [HST]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrace, H. S. (1983a) Animal cognition. In Animal Cognition, ed. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. & Terrace, H. S.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [HST]Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S. (1983b) Simultaneous chaining: The problem it poses for traditional chaining theory. In: Quantitative studies in operant behavior: Acquisition, ed. Commons, M. L., Wagner, A. R. & Herrnstein, R. J.. Ballinger. [HST]Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1913) Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review 20:158–77. [CFL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, G. (1981) Learning through interaction: Study of language development. Cambridge University Press. [RPB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitlock, F. A. (1967) The aetiology of hysteria. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 43:144–62. [DTK]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, T. (1978) Cognitive behavior therapy: Paradigm shift or passing phase? In: Cognitive behavior therapy: Research and application, ed. Foreyt, J. & Rathjen, D.. Plenum. [CFL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, L. (1976) Teleological explanations. University of California Press. [JDR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentall, T. (1983) Cognitive factors in conditional learning by pigeons. In: Animal cognition, ed. Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. & Terrace, H. S.. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [HST]Google Scholar
Zettle, R. D. & Hayes, S. C. (1982) Rule-governed behavior: A potential theoretical framework for cognitive behavior therapy. In: Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy, vol. 1, ed. Kendall, P. C.. Academic Press. [CFL]Google Scholar
Ziff, P. (1970) A response to stimulus meaning. Philosophical Review 79:63–74. [JB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar