Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T04:19:04.714Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A theory in need of defense

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 February 2010

Marc N. Branch
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, University of Florida, Gainesville, Fla. 32611

Abstract

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1982

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, D. B. (1968) Cells related to fighting behavior recorded from midbrain central gray neuropil of cat. Science 159:894–96. [DA]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adams, D. B. (1979) Brain mechanisms for offense, defense, and submission. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2:201–41. [DA, rFAM, JP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, D. B. (1980) Motivational systems of agonistic behavior in muroid rodents; A comparative review and neural model. Aggressive Behavior 6:295346. [DA, rFAM]3.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, D. B. (1981) Motivational systems of social behavior in male rats and monkeys: Are they homologous? Aggressive Behavior 7:518. [DA]3.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allison, J., Larson, D. & Jensen, D. D. (1967) Acquired fear, brightness preference, and one-way shuttle box performance. Psychonomic Science 8:269–70. [RCB, tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, J. R. (1976) Language, memory and thought. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [tarFAM]Google Scholar
Anger, D. (1963) The role of temporal discrimination in the reinforcement of Sidman avoidance behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 6:477506. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anisman, H. & Wahlsten, D. (1974) Response initiation and directionality as factors influencing avoidance performance. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 87:1119–28. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Annan, Z. & Kamin, L. J. (1961) The conditioned emotional response as a function of the intensity of the US. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 54:428–32. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Archer, J. (1979) Behavioural aspects of fear. In: Fear in animals and man, ed. Sluckin, W., pp. 5685. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Arnaut, L. & Shettleworth, S. J. (1981) The role of spatial and temporal contiguity in defensive burying in rats. Animal Learning and Behavior 9:275–80. [DT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aston-Jones, G. & Bloom, F. E. (1981) Norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats exhibit pronounced responses to non-noxious environmental stimuli. Journal of Neuroscience 1:887900. [JP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, S. A. (1958) Exploratory behaviour. British Journal of Psycholgoy 49:289310. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnett, S. A. (1975) The rat: A study in behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Baron, A., De Waard, R. J. & Lipson, J. (1977) Increased reinforcement when timeout from avoidance includes access to a safe place. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27:479–94. [MNB, rFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartter, W. D. & Masterson, F. A. (1980) Is there sign-tracking in aversive conditioning? Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 15:8789. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bastock, M., Morris, D. & Moynihan, M. (1953) Some comments on conflicts and thwarting in animals. Behaviour 6:6684. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baum, M. & Bobrow, S. (1966) An automated analogue of the one-way Miller-type avoidance box. Psychonomic Science 5:361–62. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, C. G. (1973) Species-typical behavior and ethology. In: Comparative psychology: A modern survey, ed. Dewsbury, D. A., & Rethlingshafer, D. A., pp. 2171. New York: McGraw-Hill. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Berger, D. F. & Brush, F. R. (1975) Rapid acquisition of discrete-trial lever-press avoidance: Effects of signal–shock interval. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 24:227–39. [JAD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bersh, P. J. & Alloy, L. B. (1980) Reduction of shock duration as negative reinforcement in free-operant avoidance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 33:265–73. [PJB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bersh, R. J. & Keltz, J. R. (1971) Avoidance behavior in rats after extinction with response prevention. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 76:262–66. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bersh, P. J., Whitehouse, W. G. & Mauro, B. C. (1982). Pavlovian processes and response competition as determinants of avoidance response-prevention effects. Learning and Motivation 13:113–34. [PJB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bindra, D. (1972) A unified account of classical conditioning and operant training. In: Classical conditioning II, ed. Black, A. H., & Prokasy, W. F., pp. 453–81. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Bindra, D. (1974) A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psychological Review 81:199213. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bindra, D. (1978) How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1:4191. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanchard, R. J. & Blanchard, D. C. (1969) Crouching as an index of fear. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 67:370–75. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bolles, R. C. (1970) Species-specific defensive reactions and avoidance learning. Psychological Review 77:3248. [JAD, taFAM, DT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1972a) The avoidance learning problem. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 6, ed. Bower, G. H., pp. 97145. New York: Academic Press. [tarFAM]Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1972b) Reinforcement, expectancy, and learning. Psychological Review 79:394409. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1975) Learning theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1978) The role of stimulus learning in defensive behavior. In: Cognitive processes in animal behavior, ed. Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H., & Honig, W. K., pp. 89107. Hillsdale, N.J;: Erlbaum. [RCB, tarFAM]Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1979) Learning theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. [FMT]Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. & Fanselow, M. S. (1980) A perceptual–defensive–recuperative model of fear and pain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:291323. [RCB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. & Grossen, N. E. (1969) Effects of an informational stimulus on the acquisition of avoidance behavior in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 68:9099. [AKM, rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C. & Riley, A L. (1973) Freezing as an avoidance response: Another look at the operaant–respondent distinction. Learning and Motivation 4:268–75. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bolles, R. C., Stokes, L.W. & Younger, M. S. (1966) Does CS termination reinforce avoidance behavior? Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 62:201–07. [AKM, taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, G. H., Starr, R. & Lazarovitz, L. (1965) Amount of response-produced change in the CS and avoidance learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 59:1317. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brogden, W. J., Lipman, E. A. & Culler, E.(1938) The role of incentive in conditioning and extinction. American Journal of Psychology 51:109–17. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, J. S. & Jacobs, A. (1949) The role of fear in the motivation and acqusition of responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology 39:747–59. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, S. L. (1959) Resistance to extinction as a function of number of shock-termínatíon reínforcements. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 52:754–58. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, M. M. & Galef, B. G. (1977) The role of the physical rearing environment in the domestication of the Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus). Animal Behaviour 25:298316. [DA]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Commons, M. L. (1981) How reinforcement density is discriminated. In: Quantitative analysis of behavior, vol. I, ed. Commons, M. L., & Nevin, J. A., Cambridge, Mass.: Ballinger. [rFAM]Google Scholar
Coulson, G., Coulson, V. & Gardner, L. (1970) The effect of two extinction procedures after acquisition on a Sidman avoidance contingency. Psychonomic Science 18:309–10. [PJB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coulter, X., Riccio, D. C. & Page, H. A. (1969) Effects of blocking an instrumental avoidance response: Facilitated extinction but persistence of “fear.” Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 68:377–81. [PJB]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, W. (1918) Appetites and aversions as constituents of instinct. Biological Bulletin 34:91107. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, M. (1977) Brief “response prevention” in a novel place can facilitate avoidance extinction. Learning and Motviation 8:3953. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crawford, M. & Masterson, F. (1978) Components of the flight response can reinforce bar-press avoidance learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 4:144–51. [AKM, tarFAM]Google Scholar
Crawford, M., Masterson, F. A. & Wilson, D. (1977) Species-specific defense reactions in escape-from-fear situations. Animal Learning and Behavior 5:6372. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Amato, M. R. (1967) Role of anticipatory responses in avoidance conditioning: An important control. Psychonomic Science 8:191–92. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D'Amato, M. R. (1970) Experimental psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. [AKM]Google Scholar
D'Amato, M. R., Fazzaro, J. & Etkin, M. (1968) Anticipatory responding and avoidance discrimination as factors in avoidance conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 77:4147. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Amato, M. R., Keller, D. & Biederman, G. (1965) Discriminated avoidance learning as a function of parameters of discontinuous shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology 70:543–48. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davenport, G., Coger, R. W. & Spector, N. J. (1970) The re-definition of extinction applied to Sidman free-operant avoidance responding. Psychonomic Science 19:181–82. [PJB, rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, D. E., Emlen, J. T. & Stokes, A. W. (1948) Studies on home range in the brown rat. Journal of Mammalogy 29:207–25. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, H. & Burton, J., (1976) Reinforcement of leverholding by avoidance of shock. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 8:617–20. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delprato, D. J. & Holmes, P. A. (1977) Facilitation of discriminated lever-press avoidance by noncontingent shocks. Learning and Motivation 8:238–46. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denny, M. R. (1971) Relaxation theory and experiments. In: Aversive conditioning and learning, ed. Brush, F. R., pp. 234–95. New York: Academic Press. [RCB, taFAM]Google Scholar
Deutsch, J. A. (1971) Appetite motivation. In: Psychobiology, ed. McGaugh, J. L.. New York: Academic Press, 1971. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1980) Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [FMT]Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1954) Punishment: I. The avoidance hypothesis. Psychological Review 61:3446. [taFAM)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1968) Escape from shock as a conditioning technique. In: Miami symposium on the prediction of behavior, 1967: Aversive stimulation, ed. Jones, M. R., pp. 3375. Coral Gables, Fla.: Univ. of Miami Press. [JAD]Google Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A. (1977) Escape, avoidance, punishment: Where do we stand? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 28:8395. [PNH, AKM, taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dinsmoor, J. A. & Hughes, L. H. (1956) Training rats to press a bar to turn off shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 49:235–38. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinsmoor, J. A. & Sears, G. W. (1973) Control of avoidance by a response-produced stimulus. Learning and Motivation 4:284–93. [PNH, rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, R. J. (1966) Cues for spontaneous alternation. Journal of comparative and physiological psychology 62:171–83. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1979) Human ethology: Concepts and implications for the sciences of man. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 2:158. [JP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fanselow, M. S. (1980) Conditional and unconditional components of post-shock freezing. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science 15:177–82. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fantino, E., Sharp, D. & Cole, M. (1966) Factors facilitating lever-press avoidance. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 66:214–17. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazzaro, J. & D'Amato, M. R. (1969) Resistance to extinction after varying amounts of nondiscriminative or cue-correlated escape training. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 68:373–76. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, M. W. & Cohen, J. A. (1977) Canid communication. In: How animals communicate, ed. Sebeok, T. A.. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. [rFAM]Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R. (1980) The organization of action. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum. [tarFAM]Google Scholar
Gardner, E. T. & Lewis, P. (1976) Negative reinforcement with shock-frequency increase. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 25:314. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodkin, F. (1976) Rats learn the relationship between responding and external events: An expansion of the learned helplessness hypothesis. Learning and Motivation 7:382–93. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossen, N. E. & Kelley, M. J. (1972) Species-specific behavior in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 81:307–10. [RCB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, L. W. (1972) Intrabox and extrabox cues in avoidance responding: Effect of septal lesions. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 78:268–73. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, R. C., Castellucci, V. F. & Kandel, E. R. (1981) Interneurons involved in mediation and modulation of gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. II: Identified neurons produce heterosynaptic lacilitation contributing to behavioral sensitization. Journal of Neurophysiology 45:315–26. [DA]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hearst, E. & Jenkins, H. M. (1974) Sign-tracking: The stimulus-reinforcer relation and directed action. Austin: Psychonomic Society. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1969) Method and theory in the study of avoidance. Psychological Review 76:4969. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1977) The evolution of behaviorism. American Psychologist 32:593603. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. (1979) Derivatives of matching. Psychological Review 86:486–95. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. & Hineline, P. N. (1966) Negative reinforcement as shock-frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 9:421–30. [PNH, AWL, taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinde, R. A. (1970) Animal behavior: A synthesis of ethology and comparative psychology 2nd. ed.New York: McGraw–Hill. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Hineline, P. N. (1970) Negative reinforcement without shock reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 4:259–68. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hineline, P. N. (1977) Negative reinforcement and avoidance. In: Handbook of operant behavior, ed. Honig, W. K., & Staddon, J. E. R., pp. 364414. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice–Hall. [PJB, JAD, PNH, AWL]Google Scholar
Hineline, P. N. (1981) The several roles of stimuli in negative reinforcement. In: Predictability, correlation, and contiguity, ed. Harzem, P., & Zeiler, M. D., pp. 203–46. Chichester, England: John Wiley & Sons. [PNH, rFAM]Google Scholar
Hineline, P. N. & Harrison, J. F. (1978) Lever-biting as an avoidance response. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 11:223–26. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunter, W. S. (1935) Conditioning and extinction in the rat. British Journal of Psychology 26:135–48. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Hurwitz, H. M. B., Roberts, A. E. & Greenway, L. (1972) Extinction and maintenance of avoidance behavior using response-independent shock. Psychonomic Science 28:176–78. [PJB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irwin, F. W. (1971) Intentional behavior and motivation: A cognitive theory. New York: J. B. Lippincott. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. & Masterson, F. A. (1981) Colby's paranoia model: An old theory in a new frame? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4:539–40. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, W. J. & LoLordo, V. M. (1977) The sensory basis of avoidance responding in the rat: Relative dominance of auditory or visual warning signals and safety signals. Learning and Motivation 8:448–66. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobs, W. J. & LoLordo, V. M. (1980) Constraints on Pavlovian aversive conditioning: Implications for avoidance learning in the rat. Learning and Motivation 11:427–55. (taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890) Principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt & Co. [rFAM]Google Scholar
Johnston, T. D. (1981) Contrasting approaches to a theory of learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4:125–73. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamin, L. J.(1954) Traumatic avoidance learning: The effects of CS–US interval with a trace-conditioning procedure. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 47:6572. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamin, L. J.(1956) The effects of termination of the CS and avoidance of the US on avoidance learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 49:420–24. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamin, L. J.(1957) The gradient of secondary reward in avoidance learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 50:445–49. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamin, L. J.(1969) Selective association and conditioning. In: Fundamental issues in associatve learning, ed. Mackintosh, N. J., & Honig, W. K., pp. 4264. Halifax: Dalhousie University Press. [FMT]Google Scholar
Kamin, L., Brimer, C. & Black, A. (1963) Conditioned suppression as a monitor of fear of the CS in the course of avoidance training. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 56:497501. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamin, L., Campbell, R., Judd, T., Ryan, T. & Walker, J. (1959) Two determinants of the emergence of anticipatory avoidance. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 52:202–05. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karpicke, J., Christoph, G., Peterson, G. & Hearst, E.(1977) Signal location and positive versus negative conditioned suppression in the rat. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 3:105–18. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Katzev, R. D. (1965) The extinction of avoidance responses. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley. [EJW]Google Scholar
Katzev, R. & Mills, S. (1974a) Strain differences in avoidance conditioning as a function of the classical CS–US contingency. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 87:661–71. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Katzev, R. & Mills, S. (1974b) Classical conditioning of skeletal avoidance responses in two strains of rats. Psychological Reports 35:355–60. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keehn, J. D. (1959) The effect of a warning signal on unrestricted avoidance behavior. British Journal of Psychology 50:125–35. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keltz, J. R. (1974) Effects of shock intensity, response type, and stimulus conditions on the acquisition and extinction of avoidance in the albino rat. Dissertation Abstracts International 35:3064–65. [PJB]Google Scholar
Kilmer, W. L., McCulloch, W. S. & Blum, J. (1969) A model of the vertebrate central command system. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies 1:279309. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimble, G. A.(1961) Hilgard and Marquis' conditioning and learning. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Klopfer, P. H. (1974) An introduction to animal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice–Hall. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Knapp, R. K. (1965) Acquisition and extinction of avoidance with similar and different shock and escape situations. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 60:272–73. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kortlandt, A. (1959) An attempt at clarifying some controversial notions inanimal psychology and ethology. Archives Neerlandaise de Zoologie 13:196229. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, P., Cardner, E. T. & Hutton, L. (1976) Integrated delays to shock as negative reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 26:379–86. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logue, A. W. (in press) The growth of behaviorism. In: Points of view in the modern history of psychology, ed. Buxton, C.. New York: Academic Press. [AWL]Google Scholar
Logue, A. W. & de Villiers, P. A. (1978) Matching in concurrent variable-interval avoidance schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 29:6166. [AWL]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logue, A. W. & de Villiers, P. A. (1981) Matching of behavior maintained by concurrent shock avoidance and food reinforcement. Behaviour Analysis Letters 1:247–58. [AWL]Google Scholar
Lorenz, K. (1970) The establishment of the instinct concept. (Originally published in Die Naturwissenschaften 30, 1937.) In: Studies in animal and human behaviour, vol. 1, ed. Lorenz, K., pp. 259315. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Maatsch, J. L. (1959) Learning and fixation after a single shock trial. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 52:408–10. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackintoch, N. J. (1974) The psychology of learning. New York: Academic Press. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Maier, S. F. & Seligman, M. E. P. (1975) Learned helplessness: Theory and evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 105:346. [PJB, taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marazzo, M. J., Riccio, D. C. & Riley, J. (1974) Effects of Pavlovian conditioned stimulus–unconditioned stimulus pairings during avoidance response-prevention trials in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 86:96100. [PJB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masterson, F. A. (1970). Is termination of a warning signal an effective reward for the rat? Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 72:471–75. [AKM, tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mastèrson, F. A., Crawford, M. & Bartter, W. (1978) Brief escape from a dangerous place: The role of reinforcement in the rat's one-way avoidance acquisition. Learning and Motivation 9:141–63. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Masterson, F. A., Whipple, M. C. & Benner, S. (1972) The role of proprioceptive stimulus change in the rat's avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science 27:260–62. [AKM, tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauro, B. C. & Logue, A. W. (1982) The facilitation of self-control: Fading along the dimension of shock delay. Paper presented at the meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Baltimore, 04, 1982. [AWL]Google Scholar
Mayes, A. (1979) The physiology of fear and anxiety. In: Fear in animals and man, ed. Sluckin, W.. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. [taFAM]Google Scholar
McAllister, W. R. & McAllister, D. E. (1962) Role of the CS and of apparatus cues in the measurement of acquired fear. Psychological Reports 11:749–56. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McFarland, D. J. (1974) Time-sharing as a behavioral phenomenon. In: Advances in the study of behavior, vol. 5, ed. Lehrman, D. S., Rosenblatt, J. S., Hinde, R. A., & Shaw, E., pp. 201–25. New York: Academic Press. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Meyer, D. R., Cho, C. & Wesemann, A. S. (1960) On problems of conditioning discriminated lever-press avoidance responses. Psychological Review 67:224–28. [AKM, taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1948) Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I. Fear as motivation and fear-reduction as reinforcement in the learning of new responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology 38:89101. [AKM, taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mineka, S. (1979) The role of fear in theories of avoidance learning, flooding, and extinction. Psychological Bulletin 86:9851010. [JAD, taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modaresi, H. A. (1975) One-way characteristic performance of rats under two-way signaled avoidance conditions. Learning and Motivation 6:484–97. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modaresi, H. A. (1978) Facilitating effects of a safe platform on two-way avoidance learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 4:8394. [AKM]Google Scholar
Moore, R. Y. & Bloom, F. E. (1979) Central catecholamine neuron systems: Anatomy and physiology of the norepinephrine and epinephrine systems. Annual Review of Neuroscience 2:113–68. [JP]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, M. J. (1968) Negative reinforcement. In: Analysis of behavioral change, ed. Weiskrantz, L., pp. 1949. New York: Harper and Row. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Morse, W. H. & Kelleher, R. T. (1977) Determinants of reinforcement and punishment. In: Handbook of operant behavior, ed. Honig, W. K., & Staddon, J. E. R., pp. 174200. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. [MNB]Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1947) On the dual nature of learning: A reinterpretation of “conditioning” and “problem-solving.” Harvard Educational Review 17:102–48. [tarFAM]Google Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1960a) Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mowrer, O. H. (1960b) Learning theory and the symbolic processes. New York: Wiley. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myer, J. S. (1971) Some effects of noncontingent aversive stimulation. In: Aversive conditioning and learning, ed. Brush, F. R., pp. 469536. New York: Academic Press. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, A. K. (1959) Avoidance learning as a function of several training conditions and strain differences in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 52:381–86. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Myers, A. K. (1964) Discriminated operant avoidance learning in Wistar and G-4 rats as a function of type of warning stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 58:453–55. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newell, A. (1973) Production systems: Models of control structures. In: Visual information processing, ed. Chase, W. C., pp. 463526. New York: Academic Press. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, A. & Simon, H. A. (1972) Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice–Hall. [tarFAM]Google Scholar
Obrist, P. A., Sutterer, J. R. & Howard, J. L. (1972) Preparatory cardiac changes: A psychobiological approach. In: Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory, ed. Black, A. H., & Prokasy, W. F., pp. 312–40. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Olton, D. S. & Isaacson, R. L. (1968) Importance of spatial location in active avoidance tasks. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 65:535–39. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Owen, J. W., Cicala, G. A. & Herdegen, R. T. (1978) Fear inhibition and species-specific defense reaction termination may contribute independently to avoidance learning. Learning and Motivation 9:297313. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1981) Hypothalamic integration of behavior: Rewards, punishments, and related psychological processes. In: Handbook of the hypothalamus. Vol. 3, Part B. Behavioral studies of the hypothalamus, ed. Morgane, P. J., & Panksepp, J., pp. 289431. New York: Marcel Dekker. [JP]Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1982) Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5:407–67. [rFAM, JP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearce, G. M. & Hall, G. A. (1980) A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review 87:532–52. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinel, J. P. & Treit, D. (1978) Burying as a defensive response in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 92:708–12. [DT]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinel, P. J., Treit, D., Ladak, F. & MacLennan, A. J. (1980) Conditioned definesive burying in rats free to escape. Animal Learning and Behavior, 8:447–51. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powell, R. W. (1972) Some effects of response-independent shocks after unsignalled avoidance conditioning in rats. Learning and Motivation 3:420–41. [PJB]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Powell, R. W. & Morris, G. (1968) A comparison of escape and avoidance conditioning in wild and domesticated rats. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 11:473–78. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D. (1959) Toward empirical behavioral laws: I. Positive reinforcement. Psychological Review 66:219–33. [PJB, PNH, rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rachlin, H. & Herrnstein, R. J. (1969) Hedonism revisited: On the negative law of effect. In: Punishment and aversive behavior, ed. Campbell, B., & Church, R. M., pp. 83109. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall. [AWL]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & LoLordo, V. M. (1965) Inhibition of avoidance behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 59:406–12. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A. & Solomon, R. L. (1967) Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychological Review 7:151–81. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Classical conditioning II, ed. Black, A. and Prokasy, W. F., pp. 6499. New York: Appleton–Century–Crofts. [rFAM]Google Scholar
Rosenmann, M. & Morrison, P. (1974) Physiological characteristics of the alarm reaction in the deer mouse, Peromyscus maniculatus bairdii. Physiological Zoology 47:230–41. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P. & Kalat, J. W. (1971) Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychological Review 78:459–86. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salzen, E. A. (1979) The ontogeny of fear in animals. In: Fear in animals and man, ed. Sluckin, W., pp. 125–63. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950) An experimental approach to anxiety, escape, and avoidance behavior. In: Anxiety, ed. Hoch, P. H. and Zubin, J., pp. 7099. New York: Grune & Stratton. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schlosberg, H. (1934) Conditioned responses in the white rat. Journal of Genetic Psychology 45:303–35. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Schlosberg, H. (1936) Conditioned responses in the white rat: II. Conditioned responses based on shock to the foreleg. Journal of Genetic Psychology 49:107–38. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Schlosberg, H. (1937) The relationship between success and the laws of conditioning. Psychological Review 44:379–94. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, B. (1974) On going back to nature: A review of Seligman and Hager's “Biological boundaries of learning.” Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21:183–98. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. (1970) On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review 77:406–18. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seligman, M. E. P. & Johnston, J. C. (1973) A cognitive theory of avoidance learning. In: Contemporary approaches to learning and conditioning, ed. McGuigan, F. J., & Lumsden, D. B., pp. 69145. Washington: V. H. Winston & Sons. [JAD, taFAM, FMT]Google Scholar
Sheffield, F. D. & Roby, T. B. (1950) Reward value of a non-nutritive sweet taste. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 43:471–81. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheffield, F. D., Roby, T. B. & Campbell, B. A. (1954) Drive reduction versus consummatory behavior as determinants of reinforcement. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 47:349–54. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shettleworth, S. J. (1972) Constraints on learning. In: Advances in the study of behavior, Vol. 4, ed. Lehrman, D. S., Hinde, R. A., & Shaw, E., pp. 168. New York: Academic Press. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidman, M. (1953a) Two temporal parameters of the maintenance of avoidance behavior by the white rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 46:253–61. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidman, M. (1953b) Avoidance conditioning with brief shock and no exteroceptive warning signal. Science 118:157–58. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidman, M. (1955) Some properties of the warning stimulus in avoidance behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 48:444–50. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidman, M. (1957) Conditioned reinforcing and aversive stimuli in an avoidance situation. Proceedings of the New York Academy of Sciences 19:534–44. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidman, M. (1962) Reduction of shock frequency as reinforcement for avoidance behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 5:247–57. [JAD]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sidman, M. & Boren, J. J. (1957a) A comparison of two types of warning stimulus in an avoidance situation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 50:282–87. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sidman, M. & Boren, J. J. (1957b) The relative aversiveness of warning signal and shock in an avoidance situation. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 55:339–44. [PNH]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953) Science and human behavior. New York: The Free Press. [AWL, taFAM]Google Scholar
Smith, G. D. (1973) Extinction of free-operant avoidance in rats. Temple University Ph.D. Dissertation. [PJB]Google Scholar
Solomon, R. L. & Corbit, J. D. (1974) An opponent-process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Reveiw 81:119–45. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, R. L., Kamin, L. J. & Wynne, L. C. (1953) Traumatic avoidance learning: The outcomes of several extinction procedures with dogs. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 48:291302. [PJB, rFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solomon, R. L. & Wynne, L. C. (1954) Traumatic avoidance learning: The principles of anxiety conservation and partial irreversibility. Psychological Review 61:353–85. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Staddon, J. E. R. & Simmelhag, V. L. (1971) The “superstition” experiment: A reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review 78:343. [rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starr, M. D. & Mineka, S. (1977) Determinants of fear over the course of avoidance learning. Learning and Motivation 8:332–50. [tarFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suarez, S. D. & Gallup, G. G. (1981) An ethological analysis of open field behavior in rats and mice. Learning and Motivation 12:342–63. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Testa, T. (1975) Effects of similarity of location and temporal intensity pattern of conditioned and unconditioned stimuli on the acquisition of conditioned suppression in rats. Jouranl of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 104:114–21. [rFAM]Google Scholar
Timberlake, W. (1980) A molar equilibrium theory of learned performance. In: The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 14, ed. Bower, G. H., pp. 158. New York: Academic Press. [PJB, PNH, rFAM]Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1951) The study of instinct. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [tarFAM]Google Scholar
Toates, F. M. (1981) The control of ingestive behaviour by internal and external stimuli – A theoretical review. Appetite 2:3550. [FMT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tolman, E. C. (1932) Purposive behavior in animals and men. New York: Century. [taFAM]Google Scholar
Turner, L. H. & Solomon, R. L. (1962) Human traumatic avoidance learning: Theory and experiments on the operant-respondent distinction and failures to learn. Psychological Monographs 76. (40, Whole No. 559). [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Uhl, C. N. & Eichbauer, E. A. (1975) Relative persistence of avoidance and positively reinforced behavior. Learning and Motivation 6:468–83. [JAD, rFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valenstein, E. S., Cox, V. C. & Kakolewski, J. W. (1970) Reexamination of the role of the hypothalamus in motivated behavior. Psychological Review 77:1631. [JP]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Holst, E. (1955) Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. British Journal of Animal Behaviour 2:8994. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Hulst, E. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950) Das Reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften 20:464–76. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wahlsten, D. & Sharp, D. (1969). Improvement of shuttle avoidance by handling during the intertrial interval. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 67:252–59. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weissman, A. (1962) Nondiscriminated avoidance behavior in a large sample of rats. Psychological Reports 10:591600. [AKM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiepkema, P. R. (1961) An ethological analysis of the reproductive behavior of the bitterling. Archives Neerlandaises de Zoologie 14:103–99. [taFAM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar