Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-l7hp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-28T06:02:13.562Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Music, groove, and play

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 September 2021

Richard D. Ashley*
Affiliation:
Department of Music Studies, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL60208, USA. [email protected]

Abstract

Savage et al. include groove and dance among musical features which enhance social bonds and group coherence. I discuss groove as grounded in structure and performance, and relate musical performance to play in nonhuman animals and humans. The interplay of individuals' contributions with group action is proposed as the common link between music and play as contributors to social bonding.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ashley, R. (2014). Expressiveness in funk. In Fabian, D., Timmers, R. & Schubert, E. (Eds.), Expressiveness in music performance: Empirical approaches across styles and cultures (pp. 154169). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekoff, M. (1984). Social play behavior. Bioscience, 34(4), 228233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekoff, M., & Byers, J. A. (Eds.). (1998). Animal play: Evolutionary, comparative and ecological perspectives. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bregman, A. S. (1994). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Burghardt, G. M. (2005). The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chu, J., & Schulz, L. E. (2020). Play, curiosity, and cognition. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 2, 317–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danielsen, A. (2006). Presence and pleasure: The funk grooves of James Brown and Parliament. Wesleyan University Press.Google Scholar
Graham, K. L., & Burghardt, G. M. (2010). Current perspectives on the biological study of play: Signs of progress. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 85(4), 393418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keil, C. (1987). Participatory discrepancies and the power of music. Cultural Anthropology, 2(3), 275283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spinka, M., Newberry, R. C., & Bekoff, M. (2001). Mammalian play: Training for the unexpected. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 76(2), 141168.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trainor, L. J. (2018). The origins of music: Auditory scene analysis, evolution, and culture in musical creation. In Honing, H. (Ed.), The origins of musicality (pp. 81112). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Witek, M. A., Clarke, E. F., Wallentin, M., Kringelbach, M. L., & Vuust, P. (2014). Syncopation, body-movement and pleasure in groove music. PLoS ONE, 9(4), e94446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed