No CrossRef data available.
Article contents
I've got you under my skin
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 February 2010
Abstract
An abstract is not available for this content so a preview has been provided. Please use the Get access link above for information on how to access this content.
- Type
- Open Peer Commentary
- Information
- Copyright
- Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1984
References
Attneave, F. (1960) In defense of homunculi. In: Sensory communication, ed. Rosenblith, W.. MIT Press. [WGL]Google Scholar
Barr, A., Cohen, P. R. & Feigenbaum, E. A., eds. (1982) The Handbook of Artificial Intelligence, vols. 1–3. William Kaufmann. [ML]Google Scholar
Berlin, B. & Kay, P. (1969) Basic color terms: Their universality and evolution. University of California Press. [TRZ]Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C. & Weinberg, A. (1983) The grammatical basis of linguistic performance. MIT Press. [JCM]Google Scholar
Bindra, D. (1978) How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behavioral and Brainmn Sciences 1:41–91. [FMT]Google Scholar
Block, N., ed. (1980a) Readings in the philosophy of psychology, vol. 1. Harvard University Press. [RS]Google Scholar
Block, N., ed. (1980b) Troubles with functionalism. In: Readings in the philosophy of psychology, vol. 1, ed. Block, N.. Harvard University Press. [GR]Google Scholar
Block, N., ed. (1980c) What is functionalism? In: Readings in the philosophy of psychology, vol. 1, ed. Block, N.. Harvard University Press. [LHD]Google Scholar
Blodgett, H. (1929) The effect of the introduction of reward upon the maze performance of rats. In: University of California publications in psychology. University of California Press. [GR, rBFS]Google Scholar
Blough, D. S. (1956) Dark adaptation in the pigeon. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 49:425–30. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Blough, D. S. (1959) Delayed matching in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 2:151–60. [WST]Google Scholar
Blough, D. S. & Schrier, A. M. (1963) Scotopic spectral sensitivity in the monkey. Science 139:493–94. [taBFS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H. (1975) Qualities of color vision in infancy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 19:401–19. [RKT]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, M. H. (1978) Chromatic vision in infancy. In: Advances in child development and behavior, vol. 12, ed. Reese, H. W. & Lipsitt, L. H.. Academic Press. [HMW]Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (1981) Psychological studies of color perception in human infants: Habituation, discrimination and categorization, recognition and conceptualization. In: Advances in infancy research, ed. Lipsitt, L. P. & Rovee-Collier, C. K.. Ablex. [TRZ]Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H., Kessen, W. & Weiskopf, S. (1976) Color vision and hue categorization in young human infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Performance and Perception 2:115–29. [TRZ]Google ScholarPubMed
Brady, J. V. (1975) Conditioning and emotion. In: Emotions: Their parameters and measurement, ed. Levi, L.. Raven Press. [MLW]Google Scholar
Bransford, J. D., McCarrel, N. W., Franks, J. J. & Nitsch, K. E. (1977) Towards unexplaining memory. In: Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology, ed. Shaw, R. E. & Bransford, J. D.. Erlbaum. [MJM]Google Scholar
Bretherton, I., McNew, S. & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981) Early person knowledge as expressed in gestural and verbal communication: When do infants acquire a “Theory of Mind?” In: Social cognition in infancy, ed. Lamb, M. & Sherrod, L.. Erlbaum. [CNJ]Google Scholar
Brunswik, E. (1955) Representative design and probabilistic theory in a functional psychology. Psychological Review 62:193–217. [EJW]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959) Review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Language 35:26–58. [WGL, GR, DNR]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danto, A. C. (1983) Science as an intentional system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:359–60. [MB]Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1970) Mental events. In: Experience and theory, ed. Foster, L. & Swenson, J. W.. University of Massachusetts Press. [JH]Google Scholar
Davis, L. H. (1982) Functionalism and absent qualia. Philosophical Studies 41:231–49. [LHD]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawson, M. E. (1973) Can classical conditioning occur without contingency learning? A review and evaluation of the evidence. Psychophysiology 10:82–86. [JJF]Google Scholar
Dawson, M. E. & Furedy, J. J. (1976) The role of awareness in human differential autonomic classical conditioning: The necessary gate hypothesis. Psychophysiology 13:50–53. [JJF]Google Scholar
Day, W. (1969a) On certain similarities between the philosophical investigations of Ludwig Wittgenstein and the operationism of B. F. Skinner. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 12:489–506. [MJM]Google Scholar
Day, W. (1969b) Radical behaviorism in reconciliation with phenomenology. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 12:315–28. [MJM]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. C. (1975) Why the law of effect will not go away. Journal of the Theory of Social Behaviour 5:169–87. [WGL]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1978b) Skinner skinned. In: Brainstorms. Bradford Books. [JEA, WGL, GR]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1978c) Why the law of effect won't go away. In: Brainstorms. Bradford Books. [GR]Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1983) Intentional systems in cognitive ethology: The “Panglossian paradigm” defended. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:343–90. [ML]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, J. A. (1960) The structural basis of behavior. University of Chicago Press. [FMT]Google Scholar
Dickinson, A. (1980) Contemporary animal learning theory. Cambridge University Press. [FMT]Google Scholar
Domjan, M & Burkhard, B. (1982) The principles of learning and behavior. Brooks-Cole. [WST]Google Scholar
Donahoe, J. W. & Wessells, M. G. (1980) Learning, language and memory. Harper & Row. [WST]Google Scholar
Duncan, P. M. & Duncan, N. C. (1971) Free-operant and T-maze avoidance performance by septal and hippocampal-damaged rats. Physiology & Behavior 7:687–93. [MLW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eddington, A. (1958) The nature of the physical world. University of Michigan Press. [EJW]Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1968a) The appeal to tacit knowledge in psychological explanation. Journal of Philosophy 65:627–40. [WGL]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1980) Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3:63–73. [WGL, JCM]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furedy, J. J. (1973) Some limits of the cognitive control of conditioned autonomic behavior. Psychophysiology 10:108–11. [JJF]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Furedy, J. J. & Riley, D. M. (1982) Classical and operant conditioning in the enhancement of biofeedback: Specifics and speculations. In: Clinical biofeedback: Efficacy and mechanisms, ed. White, L. & Tursky, B.. Guilford Press. [JJF]Google Scholar
Furedy, J. J., Riley, D. M. & Fredrickson, M. (1983) Pavlovian extinction, phobias, and the limits of the cognitive paradigm. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Psychology 18:126–35. [JJF]Google ScholarPubMed
Gallup, G. G. Jr (1983) Toward a comparative psychology of mind. In: Animal cognition and behavior, ed. Mellgren, R. L.. North Holland Publishing Co. [GGG]Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1883) Inquiries into human faculty and its development. J. M. Dent and Company. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Gelman, R. & Spelke, E. (1981) The development of thoughts about animate and inanimate objects: Implications for research on social cognition. In: Social cognitive development, ed. Flavell, J. H. & Ross, L.. Cambridge University Press. [CNJ]Google Scholar
Gier, N. F. (1981) Wittgenstein and phenomenology. State University of New York Press. [MJM]Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1980) The panda's thumb: More reflections in natural history. W.W. Norton & Co. [EJW]Google Scholar
Griffin, D. R. (1976) The question of animal awareness. Rockefeller University Press. [WST]Google Scholar
Gunderson, K. (1971) Asymmetries and mind-body perplexities. In: Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science, vol. 4, ed. Radner, M. & Winokur, S.. University of Minnesota Press. [KG]Google Scholar
Guttman, N. (1956) The pigeon and the spectrum and the other perplexities. Psychological Reports 2:449–60. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harlow, H. F. (1949) The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review 56:51–65. [GR]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harlow, H. F. (1959) Learning set and error factor theory. In: Psychology: A study of science, vol. 2, ed. Koch, S.. McGraw Hill. [GR]Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (1982) Consciousness: An afterthought. Cognition and Brain Theory 5:29–47. [DP]Google Scholar
Harzem, P. & Miles, T. R. (1978) Conceptual issues in operant psychology. John Wiley and Sons. [MJM]Google Scholar
Heider, E. R. (1972) Universals in color naming and memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology 93:10–20. [TRZ]Google Scholar
Herrnstein, R. J. & van Sommers, P. (1962) Method for sensory scaling with animals. Science 135:40–41. [taBFS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Honig, W. K. (1978) On the conceptual nature of cognitive terms: An initial essay. In: Cognitive processes in animal behavior, ed. Hulse, S. W., Fowler, H. & Honig, W. K.. Erlbaum. [WST]Google Scholar
Hospers, J. (1958) What means this freedom? In: Determinism and freedom, ed. Hook, S.. Collier Books. [JEA]Google Scholar
Hull, C. L. (1943) Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory. Appleton-Century. [EJW]Google Scholar
Hulse, S. W., Fowler, H & Honig, W. K. (1978) Cognitive processes in animal behavior. Erlbaum. [WST]Google Scholar
Irwin, F. W. (1971) Intentional behavior and motivation: A cognitive theory. J.B. Lippincott Co. [FWI]Google Scholar
Jarvik, M. E. (1956) Simple color discrimination in chimpanzees: Effects of varying continguity between cue and incentive. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 49:492–95. [EJW]Google Scholar
Jenkins, H. M. (1979) Animal learning and behavior theory. In: The first century of experimental psychology, ed. Hearst, E.. Erlbaum. [EJW]Google Scholar
Johnson, C. N. (1982) Acquisition of mental verbs and the concept of mind. In: Language development, vol. 1, ed. Kuczaj, S.. Erlbaum. [CNJ]Google Scholar
Johnson, C. N. & Wellman, H. M. (1980) Children's developing understanding of mental verbs: Remember, know, and guess. Child Development 51:1095–1102. [HMW]Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1970) The structure of scientific revolutions. University of Chicago Press. [MB]Google Scholar
Lashley, K. (1923) The behavioristic interpretation of consciousness. Psychological Review 30:237–72, 329–53. [WL]Google Scholar
Lebowitz, M. (1983) Generalization from natural language text. Cognitive Science 7:1–40. [ML]Google Scholar
Leibniz, G. (1714) The monadology and other philosophical writings. Trans. Latta, R.. Oxford University Press. [KG, JCM]Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. (1979) Sociobiology as an adaptationist paradigm. Behavioral Science 24:5–14. [EJW]Google Scholar
Lycan, W. G. (1981b) Toward a homuncular theory of believing. Cognition and Brain Theory 4:139–59. [WGL]Google Scholar
MacCorquodale, K. (1970) On Chomsky's review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 13:83–99. [JCM]Google Scholar
Marshall, J. C. (1977) Minds, machines and metaphors. Social Studies of Science 7:475–88. [JCM]Google Scholar
Maze, J. R. (1953) On some corruptions of the doctrine of homeostasis. Psychological Review 60:405–12. [JJF]Google Scholar
Menzel, E. W. (1978) Cognitive mapping in chimpanzees. In: Cognitive processes in animal behavior, ed. Hulse, S. W., Fowler, H. & Honig, W. K.. Erlbaum. [WST]Google Scholar
Menzel, E. W. Jr & Juno, C. (1982) Marmosets (Saguinus fuscicollis): Are learning sets learned? Science 217:750–52. [EJW]Google Scholar
Minsky, M. (1968) Matter, mind, and models. In: Semantic information processing, ed. Minsky, M.. MIT Press. [DP]Google Scholar
Moore, B. R. (1973) The role of directed Pavlovian reactions in simple instrumental learning in the pigeon. In: Constraints on learning, ed. Hinde, R. A. & Stevenson-Hinde, J.. Academic Press. [FMT]Google Scholar
Mountjoy, P. P. & Malott, M. K. (1968) Wave-length generalization curves for chickens reared in restricted portions of the spectrum. Psychological Record 18:575–83. [TRZ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neurath, O. (1970) Foundations of the social sciences. In: The foundations of the unity of science, ed. Neurath, O. et al. University of Chicago Press. [MB]Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. & Wilson, T. D. (1977) On saying more than we can know. Psychological Review 84:231–59. [GR]Google Scholar
Oatley, K. (1973) Simulation and theory of thirst. The neuropsychology of thirst: New findings and advances in concepts, ed. Epstein, A. N., Kissileff, H. R. & Stellar, E.. V.H. Winston. [FMT]Google Scholar
O'Keefe, J. & Nadel, L. (1978) The hippocampus as a cognitive map. Oxford University Press. [MLW]Google Scholar
Olton, D. S. (1978) Characteristics of spatial memory. In: Cognitive processes in animal behavior, ed. Hulse, S. W., Fowler, H. & Honig, W. K.. Erlbaum. [WST]Google Scholar
Olton, D. S., Becker, J. T. & Handelmann, G. E. (1979) Hippocampus, space, and memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2:313–65. [MLW]Google Scholar
Penfield, W. (1969) Consciousness, memory and man's conditioned reflexes. In: On the biology of learning, ed. Pribram, K. H.. Harcourt, Brace and Jovanovich. [FMT]Google Scholar
Peterson, N. (1962) Effect of monochromatic rearing on the control of responding by wavelength. Science 136:774–75. [rBFS]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D. & Woodruff, G. (1978) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4:515–26. [GGG, LCNJ]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pribran, K. H. (1971) Languages of the brain: Experimental paradoxes and principles in neuropsychology. Prentice-Hall. [MLW]Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975) The meaning of “meaning.” In: Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science, vol. 7, Language, mind and knowledge, ed. Gunderson, K.. University of Minnesota Press. [WGL]Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1951) Notes on the theory of reference. In: From a logical point of view and other essays. Harper & Row. [GR]Google Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1953) Two dogmas of empiricism. In: From a logical point of view. Harvard University Press. [MB]Google Scholar
Ratliff, F. (1962) Some interrelations among physics, physiology, and psychology in the study of vision. In Psychology: A study of a science, study 2, vol. 4, ed. Koch, S.. McGraw-Hill. [RKT]Google Scholar
Rescorla, R. A. & Wagner, A. R. (1972) A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In: Classical conditioning, vol. 2, ed. Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [WST]Google Scholar
Rey, G. (1980) What are mental images? In: Readings in the philosophy of psychology, vol. 2, ed. Block, N.. Harvard University Press. [GR]Google Scholar
Rey, G. (1983) A reason for doubting the existence of consciousness. In: Consciousness and self-regulation, vol. 3, ed. Davidson, R., Schwartz, G. & Shapiro, D.. Plenum. [GR]Google Scholar
Riley, D. A. & Leuin, T. C. (1971) Stimulus-generalization gradients in chickens reared in monochromatic light and tested with single wavelength values. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 75:399–402. [TRZ]Google Scholar
Robinson, D. N. (1979) Systems of modern psychology: A critical sketch. Columbia University Press. [DNR]Google Scholar
Robinson, D. N. (1981) An intellectual history of psychology. Rev. ed.Macmillan. [DNR]Google Scholar
Roediger, H. L. (1980) Memory metaphors in cognitive psychology. Memory and Cognition 8:231–46. [MJM]Google Scholar
Roitblat, H. L. (1982) The meaning of representation in animal memory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 5:353–406. [EJW]Google Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gay, W. D., Boyes-Braem, P. & Johnson, D. N. (1976) Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 8:382–439. [HMW]Google Scholar
Ross, L. (1981) The “intuitive scientist” formulation and its developmental implications. In: Social cognitive development, ed. Flavell, J. H. & Ross, L.. Cambridge University Press. [CNJ]Google Scholar
Scriven, M. (1956) A study of radical behaviorism. In: The foundations of science and the concepts of psychology and psychoanalysis. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science, vol. 1, ed. Feigl, H. & Scriven, M.. University of Minnesota Press. [MAS]Google Scholar
Shatz, M., Wellman, H. M. & Silber, S. (1983) The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state. Cognition 14:301–21. [HMW]Google Scholar
Shepard, J. (1933) Higher processes in the behavior of rats. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 19:149–52. [GR]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1938) The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [FWI]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1947) Current trends in experimental psychology. In: Current trends in psychology. University of Pittsburgh Press. [JCM]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1950) Arc theories of learning necessary? Psychological Review 57:193–216. [WST]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1965) Science and human behavior. 1st ed. 1953. Collier Macmillan-Free Press. [WL]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1966a) Operant behavior. In: Operant behavior: Areas of research and application, ed. Honig, W. K.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [MLW]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1966b) The phylogeny and ontogeny of behavior. Science 153:1205–13. [EJW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1969) Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical analysis. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [FW]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1975a) The shaping of phylogenic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 24:117–20. [RS]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1975b) The steep and thorny way to a science of behavior. In: Problems of scientific revolution, ed. Harré, R.. Clarendon Press. [BAF]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1977) Why I am not a cognitive psychologist. Behaviorism 5(2):1–10. [RKT]Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1981) Selection by consequences. Science 213:501–4. [EJW]CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skinner, B. F. (1983) A better way to deal with selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6:377–78. [ML]Google Scholar
Smart, J. (1959) Sensations and brain processes. Philosophical Review 68:141–56. [CM, GR]Google Scholar
Stevens, S. S. (1935) The operational basis of psychology. American Journal of Psychology 47:323–30. [taBFS]Google Scholar
Strauss, M. S. & Curtis, L. E. (1981) Infant perception of numerosity. Child Development 52:1146–52. [HMW]Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1968) Freedom and resentment. In: Studies in the philosophy of thought and action, ed Strawson, P. F.. Oxford University Press. [JEA, rBFS]Google Scholar
Teitelbaum, P. (1966) The use of operant methods in the assessment and control of motivational states. In: Operant behavior: Areas of research and application, ed. Honig, W. K.. Appleton-Century-Crofts. [MLW]Google Scholar
Teuber, H. L. (1974) Key problems in the programming of movements. Brain Research 71:535–68. [JCM]Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1922) A new formula for behaviorism. Psychological Review 29:44–53. [WL]Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1935) Psychology versus immediate experience. Philosophy of Science 2:356–80. [EJW]Google Scholar
Tolman, E. C. (1948) Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review 55:189–208. [EJW]Google Scholar
Tolman, E. & Honzik, C. (1930), “Insight” in rats. In: University of California publications in psychology. University of California Press. [GR, rBFS]Google Scholar
Tracy, W. K. (1970) Wavelength generalization and preference in monochromatically reared ducklings. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 13:163–78. [TRZ]Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1979) Memory research: What kind of progress. In: Perspectives in memory research, ed. Nilsson, L. A.. Erlbaum. [MJM]Google Scholar
Wagner, A. R., Rudy, J. W. & Whitlow, J. W. (1973) Rehearsal in animal conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology 97:407–26. [WST]Google Scholar
Waltz, D. L. (1975) Understanding line drawings of scenes with shadows. In: The psychology of computer vision, ed. Winston, P.. McGraw-Hill. [RCM]Google Scholar
Wann, T. W. (1964) Behaviorism and phenomenology: Contrasting bases for modern psychology. University of Chicago Press. [CNJ, RKT]Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1921) Is thinking merely that action of the language mechanisms? British Journal of Psychology 11:87–104. [WL]Google Scholar
Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E. K., Sanders, M. D. & Marshall, J. (1974) Visual capacity in the hemianopie field following a restricted occipital ablation. Brain 97:709–28. [RMG]Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M. (in press) The origins of metacognition. In: Metacognition, cognition and human performance, ed. Forrest-Pressley, D., MacKinnon, G. E. & Waller, T. G.. Academic Press. [CNJ, HMW]Google Scholar
Wiener, N. (1948) Cybernetics, or control and communication in the animal and the machine. Wiley. [JCM]Google Scholar
Wimmer, H. & Perner, J. (1983) Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition 13:103–28. [CNJ]Google Scholar
Wolf, D. (1982) Understanding others: A longitudinal case study of the concept of independent agency. In: Action and thought, ed. Forman, G. E.. Academic Press. [CNJ]Google Scholar
Woodruff, G. & Premack, D. (1979) Intentional communication in the chimpanzee: The development of deception. Cognition 7:333–62. [CNJ]Google Scholar
Wright, A. A. & Cumming, W. W. (1971) Color-naming functions for the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 15:7–17. [TRZ]Google Scholar
Zentall, T. R. & Edwards, C. A. (in press) Categorical color coding by pigeons. Animal Learning and Behavior. [TRZ]Google Scholar
Zentall, T. R., Edwards, C. A., Moore, B. S. & Hogan, D. E. (1981) Identity: The basis for both matching and oddity learning in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes 7:70–86. [TRZ]Google Scholar