Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T20:59:17.452Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Intellectual Development of Twins — Comparison with Singletons

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 August 2014

Ntinos C. Myrianthopoulos*
Affiliation:
National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA
Paul L. Nichols
Affiliation:
National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA
Sarah H. Broman
Affiliation:
National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland, USA
*
NINCDS, NIH, Bethesda, Maryland 20014, USA

Abstract

Core share and HTML view are not available for this content. However, as you have access to this content, a full PDF is available via the ‘Save PDF’ action button.

Analysis of mental and motor tests scores and intelligence test performance of twins born in the Collaborative Perinatal Project shows that twins perform more poorly than singletons from the same population and that the differences are greater in Negroes than in whites. The poor performance of twins relative to that of singletons is of complex etiology. It is partly due to poor prenatal environment, for twins brought up as singletons perform at the intelligence level of twins and not of singletons. It may also be partly due to the higher incidence of congenital malformations in twins, especially those of the central nervous system. But the performance of twins, relative to that of singletons, tends to improve as they get older, at least from 4 to 7 years, suggesting that prematurity is also a contributing factor, whose detrimental effects may be reversible.

Type
9. Science for Twins/Twins for Science
Copyright
Copyright © The International Society for Twin Studies 1976

References

REFERENCES

Broman, S.H., Nichols, P.L., Kennedy, W.A. (1975). Preschool IQ: Prenatal and Early Developmental Correlates. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Drillien, C.M. 1961. A longitudinal study of the growth and development of prematurely and maturely born children. Part VII: Mental development 2-5 years. Arch. Dis. Child., 36: 233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myrianthopoulos, N.C., French, K.S. 1968. An application of the U.S. Bureau of the Census socioeconomic index to a large, diversified patient population. Soc. Sci. Med., 2: 283.Google Scholar
Myrianthopoulos, N.C. 1970. An epidemiologic survey of twins in a large, prospectively studied population. Amer J. Hum. Genet., 22: 611 Google Scholar
Myrianthopoulos, N.C. 1974. Congenital malformations in twins. Proc. 1st. Int. Congr. Twin Studies, Rome. Acta Genet. Med. Gemellol. (Roma), 25: 331335.Google Scholar
Record, R.G., McKeown, T., Edwards, J.H. 1970. An investigation of the difference in measured intelligence between twins and single births. Ann. Hum. Genet., 34: 11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandon, F. 1957. The relative numbers and abilities of ten-year-old twins. J. Roy. Stat. Soc., 120:440.Google Scholar
Vandenberg, S.G. 1968. The nature and nurture of intelligence. In Glass, D.G. (ed): Genetics [Chapter 1]. New York: Rockfeller University Press and Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar