Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-10T04:44:47.178Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Leslie Smith
Affiliation:
Lancaster University
Jacques Vonèche
Affiliation:
Université de Genève
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anastasi, A. (1982). Psychological testing. 5th edition. New York: Macmillan Publishing Co. Inc.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. A. (1957). Significance of growth and development to the practicing physician. In Harris (1957).
Armstrong, L. (2003). Every second counts: from recovery to victory. London: Yellow Jersey Press.Google Scholar
Axtell, G. (2000). Knowledge, belief, and character: readings in virtue epistemology. New York: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers Inc.Google Scholar
Bainton, R. H. (1950). Here I stand: a life of Martin Luther. New York: Signet.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. (2005). www.inspirationalstories.com/0/91.html
Bickhard, M. (2003). Process and emergence: normative function and representation. In Seibt, J. (ed.), Process theories: crossdisciplinary studies in dynamic categories. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. & Pellegrini, A. (2002). The origins of human nature. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Blank, G. (2002). www.drblank.com/slaws.htm
Bornstein, M. & Lamb, M. (1992). Developmental psychology: an advanced textbook. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brandom, R. (1994). Making it explicit: reasoning, representing, and discursive commitment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brandom, R. (2000). Articulating reasons: an introduction to inferentialism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, T. & Weiss, L. (1987). Structures, procedures, heuristics and affectivity. Archives de Psychologie, 55, 59–94. Reprinted in L. Smith (1992), Jean Piaget: critical assessments, vol. IV. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Changeux, J-P. (2000). Co-author. In Changeux, J-P. & Ricoeur, P., What makes us think?Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cole, M., Cole, S. & Lightfoot, C. (2005). The development of children. 5th edition. New York: Worth Publishing.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (2004). What is evolutionary psychology? Explaining the new science of the mind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Cresswell, M. & Hughes, G. (1996). A new introduction to modal logic. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
Damon, W. (1998). Handbook of child psychology. 5th edition. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1999). The extended phenotype: the long reach of the gene. Revised edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1901). The rules of sociological method. In Lukes, S. (1982). London: Macmillan Press Ltd.Google Scholar
Flavell, J., Miller, P. & Miller, S. (1993). Cognitive development. 3rd edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1964). The basic laws of arithmetic. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1982). Developmental psychology: an introduction. Boston: Little, Brown & Co.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Selten, R. (2001). Bounded rationality: the adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (2001). Pathways of knowledge: public and private. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. (2002). Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habermas, J. (1992). Between facts and norms: contributions to a discourse theory of law and democracy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Harris, D. B. (1957). The concept of development. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Hawking, S. (2001). The universe in a nutshell. London: Bantam Press.Google Scholar
Horty, J. F. (2001). Agency and deontic logic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, D. (1965). Treatise of human nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Humphrey, G. (1963). Thinking. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Isaacs, N. (1951). Critical notice. British Journal of Psychology, 42, 185–8.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1780). On universal practical philosophy. In Heath, P. & Schneewind, J. (1997), Lectures on ethics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1785). Groundwork of the metaphysics of morals. In Paton, H. J. (1948), The moral law. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1800). Introduction to logic. In Abbott, T. K. (1972), Kant's Introduction to logic. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
King, M. L. (1968). The role of the behavioural scientist in the Civil Rights movement. American Psychologist, 23, 180–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1992). The naturalists return. The Philosophical Review, 101, 53, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korsgaard, C. (1996). The sources of normativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (2000). The road beyond structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Laudan, L. (1996). Beyond positivism and relativism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
LeTour (2005). www.letour.fr/indexus.html
Longuenesse, B. (1998). Kant and the capacity to judge. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, A. (1998). A short history of ethics. 2nd edition. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. (1998). Babies think before they speak. Human Development, 41, 116–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, R. B. (1993). Modalities: philosophical essays. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mason, H. E. (1996). Moral dilemmas and moral theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: an experimental view. London: Tavistock Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
Milgram, S. (1992). Individual in a social world. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Mooney, C. (2003). www.csicop.org/doubtandabout/polling/
Nadel, I. (2002). Double act: a life of Tom Stoppard. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1998). The rising curve. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Nozick, R. (2001). Invariances. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialecticism, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1918). Recherche. Lausanne: La Concorde.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1953). Origins of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1976). The grasp of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. (1997). Evolution in mind. London: Allen Lane Press.Google Scholar
Popper, K. R. (1968). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1979). Objective knowledge. 2nd edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pufendorf, S. (1703). Of the law of nature and nations. Oxford: Lichfield.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (2002). The collapse of the fact/value dichotomy, and other essays. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Quine, W. (1972). Methods of logic. 3rd edition. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, P. (2000). Co-author. In Changeux, J-P. & Ricoeur, P., What makes us think?Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ross, A. (1968). Directives and norms. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Sainsbury, R. M. (1991). Logical forms. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. (2007). 30 years on – a large anti-‘Flynn effect’? The Piagetian test volume & heaviness norms 1975–2003. Brilish Journal of Educational Psychology.CrossRef
Siegler, R. S. (1991). Children's thinking. 2nd edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1981). Sciences of the artificial. 2nd edition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviourism. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
Slater, A. & Muir, D. (1999). Blackwell reader in developmental psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (1993). Necessary knowledge. Hove, UK: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2002). Reasoning by mathematical induction in children's arithmetic. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2003). From epistemology to psychology in the development of knowledge. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spencer, R. (1957). Evolution and development: a view of anthropology. In Harris (1957).
Spinoza, B. (1963). Short treatise. New York: Russell & Russell.Google Scholar
Summers, L. H. (2005). Remarks at NBER conference on diversifying the science & engineering workforce. President, Harvard University homepage.
Wason, P. (1977). Self-contradictions. In Johnson-Laird, P. & Wason, P. (eds.), Thinking: readings in cognitive science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism. New York: Norton & Company Inc.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1963). Norm and action. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1968). The varieties of goodness. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1983). Practical reason. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
, Aristotle (1928). περι ψυξήγ. In Oeuvres complètes. Paris: Guillaume Budé.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Development and evolution. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1906, 1908, 1911). Thoughts and things or genetic logic. 3 vols. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bergson, H. (1911). Creative evolution (trans. A. Mitchell). New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Bosanquet, B. (1888). On the morphology of knowledge. 2 vols. London/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cassirer, E. (1903–20). Das Erkenntnisproblem. 3 vols. Berlin: B. Cassirer.Google Scholar
Cassirer, E. (1953, 1955, 1957). Philosophy of symbolic forms. 3 vols. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: Murray.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1897). Le suicide. Paris: Félix Alcan.Google Scholar
Furth, H. (1986). The social function of Piaget's theory: a response to Apostel. New Ideas, 4 (1), 23–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobhouse, L. T. (1888). Theory of knowledge. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hobhouse, L. T. (1912). Mind in evolution. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1881). Science and culture. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt, vol. II.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1972). Introduction to logic. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
Liard, L. (1873). De Democrito philosopho. Paris.Google Scholar
Lovejoy, A. O. (1936). The great chain of being. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. R. & Varela, F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1953). The origins of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1928). Die Methodenfrage der Denkpsychologie. Vienna: unpublished doctoral dissertation, Vienna University.
Russell, E. (1946). The defectiveness of organic activities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sigwart, C. (1894–5). Logic. 2 vols. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Thompson, D. A. W. (1942). On growth and form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1983). Practical reason. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1880). Logik. Stuttgart: Enke.Google Scholar
Aristotle (1908). De anima (On the soul). In Ross, W. D. (ed.), The works of Aristotle. Oxford: Clarendon Press, §424a, pp. 17–22.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1980). A model of developmental and psychological processes. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 102, 61–116.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1988). Piaget on variation and selection models: structuralism, logical necessity, and interactivism. Human Development, 31, 274–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1989). The nature of psychopathology. In Simek-Downing, Lynn (ed.), International psychotherapy: theories, research, and cross-cultural implications. Westport, CT: Praeger Press, pp. 115–40.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1992a). How does the environment affect the person? In Winegar, L. T. & Valsiner, J. (eds.), Children's development within social contexts: metatheory and theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 63–92.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1992b). Scaffolding and self scaffolding: central aspects of development. In Winegar, L. T. & Valsiner, J. (eds.), Children's development within social contexts: research and methodology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 33–52.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1992c). Piaget on variation and selection models: structuralism, logical necessity, and interactivism. In Smith, L. (ed.), Jean Piaget: critical assessments, vol. IV. London: Routledge, ch. 83, pp. 388–434. (Reprint of 1988)Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1992d). Commentary on the age 4 transition. Human Development, 35 (3), 182–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1993). Representational content in humans and machines. Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, 5, 285–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2000a). Motivation and emotion: an interactive process model. In Ellis, R. D. & Newton, N. (eds.), The caldron of consciousness. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins, pp. 161–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2000b). Emergence. In Andersen, P. B., Emmeche, C., Finnemann, N. O. & Christiansen, P. V. (eds.), Downward causation. Aarhus, Denmark: University of Aarhus Press, pp. 322–48.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2001). Why children don't have to solve the frame problems: cognitive representations are not encodings. Developmental Review, 21, 224–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2003a). An integration of motivation and cognition. In Smith, L., Rogers, C. & Tomlinson, P. (eds.), Development and motivation: joint perspectives. Leicester: British Psychological Society, Monograph Series II, pp. 41–56.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2003b). Process and emergence: normative function and representation. In Seibt, J. (ed.), Process theories: crossdisciplinary studies in dynamic categories. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic, pp. 121–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2003c). The biological emergence of representation. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 105–31.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (in preparation). The whole person.
Bickhard, M. H. & Campbell, R. L. (1989). Interactivism and genetic epistemology. Archives de Psychologie, 57 (221), 99–121.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. & Campbell, R. L. (1996). Topologies of learning and development. New Ideas in Psychology, 14 (2), 111–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. & Richie, D. M. (1983). On the nature of representation: a case study of James Gibson's theory of perception. New York: Praeger Publishers.Google Scholar
Brady, G. (2000). From Peirce to Skolem. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1974). Evolutionary epistemology. In Schilpp, P. A. (ed.), The Philosophy of Karl Popper. LaSalle, IL: Open Court, pp. 413–63.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1990). Levels of organization, downward causation, and the selection-theory approach to evolutionary epistemology. In G. Greenberg, & Tobach, E. (eds.), Theories of the evolution of knowing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 1–17.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. (1992). Truth and historicity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. L. & Bickhard, M. H. (1986). Knowing levels and developmental stages. Contributions to Human Development. Basel, Switzerland: Karger.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. L. & Bickhard, M. H. (1992). Types of constraints on development: an interactivist approach. Developmental Review, 12 (3), 311–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caston, V. (1997). Epiphenomenalisms, ancient and modern. Philosophical Review, 106 (3), 309–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, W. D. & Bickhard, M. H. (2002). The process dynamics of normative function. Monist, 85 (1), 3–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coffa, J. A. (1991). The semantic tradition from Kant to Carnap. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1987). A situated grandmother?Mind and Language, 2, 64–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1990a). Information and representation. In Hanson, P. P. (ed.), Information, language, and cognition. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press, pp. 175–90.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1990b). Fodor's guide to mental representation. In Fodor, J. A. (ed.), A theory of mental content and other essays. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2003). Hume variations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gill, M-L. (1989). Aristotle on substance. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hume, D. (1978). A treatise of human nature. Index by L. A. Selby-Bigge; Notes by P. H. Nidditch. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hylton, P. (1990). Russell, idealism, and the emergence of analytic philosophy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: intentional behavior as a complex system. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Olson, K. R. (1987). An essay on facts. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in reflecting abstraction, ed. and trans. Robert L. Campbell. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Plato (1892). Theaetetus. In The dialogues of Plato, vol. IV, ed. and trans. B. Jowett. Oxford: Clarendon Press, p. 191.
Rouse, J. (2002). How scientific practices matter: reclaiming philosophical naturalism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Alston, W. (1989). Epistemic justification. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Apostel, L. et al. (1957). Les liaisons analytiques et synthétiques dans les comportements du sujet. In Etudes d'épistémologie génétique, vol. IV. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Aristotle (1984). Nichomachean ethics. In Barnes, J. (ed.), The complete works of Aristotle, vol. II. Princeton: Princeton University Press, pp. 1, 729–867.Google Scholar
Armstrong, D. (1973). Belief, truth and knowledge. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Axtell, G. (2000). Knowledge, belief, and character. Lanham, MD: Rowman.Google Scholar
Bicchieri, C. (1993). Rationality and coordination. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bonjour, L. (1985). The structure of empirical knowledge. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Chisholm, R. (1977). Theory of knowledge. 2nd edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. (1979). Scientific realism and the plasticity of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, W. (1999). The ethics of belief and other essays. Amherst, NY: Prometheus.Google Scholar
Conee, E. & Feldman, R. (2004). Evidentialism. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Copp, D. (2001). Morality, normativity, and society. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dancy, J. & Sosa, E. (eds.) (1992). A companion to epistemology. Oxford: Blackwell's.Google Scholar
Danielson, P. (1992). Artificial morality. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1984). Meditations on first philosophy. In Cottingham, J. et al. (eds., trans.), The philosophical writings of Descartes, vol. II. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–62. (Original work published 1641.)Google Scholar
Fairweather, A. & Zagzebski, L. (eds.) (2001). Virtue epistemology: essays on epistemic virtue and responsibility. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gauthier, D. (1986). Morals by agreement. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Ginet, C. (1975). Knowledge, perception and memory. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (l986). Epistemology and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRef
Goldman, A. I. (1992). Liaisons: philosophy meets the cognitive and social sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (2001). Social epistemology. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/epistemology-social
Hamlyn, D. (1971). Epistemology and conceptual development. In Mischel, T. (ed.), Cognitive development and epistemology. New York: Academic Press, pp. 3–24.
Inhelder, B. (1981). Some aspects of Piaget's genetic approach to cognition. In Furth, H. (ed.), Piaget and knowledge: theoretical foundations. 2nd edition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 22–39.Google Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (1986). Piaget's theory of knowledge: genetic epistemology and scientific reason. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (1999). The conduct of inquiry: an introduction to logic and scientific method. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.Google Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (ed.) (ed.) (2000). Special issue: Piaget's Sociological studies. New Ideas In Psychology, 18, 119–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (2003). Piaget's social epistemology. In Carpendale, J. & Müller, U. (eds.), Social interaction and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 45–67.Google Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (forthcoming). Developmental epistemology: cognitive development and naturalistic epistemology. (Book manuscript.)
Kitcher, P. (1992). The naturalists return. Philosophical Review, 101, 53–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kripke, S. A. (1982). Wittgenstein on rules and private language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Laudan, L. (1987). Progress or rationality? The prospects for normative naturalism. American Philosophical Quarterly, 24, 19–31.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1975). An essay concerning human understanding (ed. P. H. Niddith, 4th edition). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Maffie, J. (1990). Recent work on naturalized epistemology. American Philosophical Quarterly, 27, 281–93.Google Scholar
Opp, D. (1983). Die Enstehung der sozialer Normen. Tübingen: J. C. B. Mohr.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1942). Les trois structures fondamentales de la vie psychique: rythme, régulation et groupement. Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie und ihre Anwendungen, 1, 9–21.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1950). Introduction à l'épistémologie génétique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, vol. I.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1955). Les lignes générales de l'épistémologie génétique. In Actes du deuxième Congrès international de l'Union internationale de philosophie des sciences, Zurich, 1954, vol. I. Neuchâtel: de Griffon, pp. 26–45.
Piaget, J. (1957). Programme et méthodes de l'épistémologie génétique. In Etudes d'épistémologie génétique, vol. I. Epistémologie génétique et recherche psychologique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, pp. 13–84.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Défense de l'épistémologie génétique. In Etudes d'épistémologie génétique, vol. XVI. Implication, formalization et logique naturelle. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, pp. 165–91.
Piaget, J. (1967a). Nature et méthodes de l'épistémologie. In Piaget, J. (ed.), Logique et connaissance scientifique. Paris: Gallimard, pp. 3–61.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1967b). Les méthodes de l'épistémologie. In Piaget, J. (ed.), Logique et connaissance scientifique. Paris: Gallimard, pp. 62–126.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971a). Psychology and epistemology. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971b). Genetic epistemology. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1972a). Insights and illusions of philosophy (trans. W. Mays, 2nd edition). New York: World Publishing Co. (Original work published 1968.)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1972b). The principles of genetic epistemology (trans. W. Mays). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1970.)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structures: the central problem of intellectual development (trans. T. Brown & K. J. Thampy). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1975.)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1986). Essay on necessity (trans. L. Smith & F. Steel). Human development, 29, 301–14.CrossRef
Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies (ed. L. Smith, trans. L. Smith et al.). London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Plantinga, A. (1993a). Warrant and proper function. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plantinga, A. (1993b). Warrant: the current debate. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1951). Two dogmas of empiricism. Philosophical Review, 60, 20–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1969). Epistemology naturalized. In his Ontological relativity and other essays. New York: Columbia University Press, pp. 69–90.Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schmitt, F. (1994). Socializing epistemology: an introduction through two sample issues. In Schmitt, F. (ed.), Socializing epistemology: the social dimensions of knowledge. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield, pp. 1–27.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1993). Necessary knowledge. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1995). Introduction. In Smith, L. (ed., trans. L. Smith et al.), Jean Piaget: sociological studies. London: Routledge, pp. 1–22.Google Scholar
Sosa, E. (1991). Knowledge in perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugden, R. (1986). The economics of rights, cooperation and welfare. Oxford: Blackwell's.Google Scholar
Taylor, M. (1984). The possibility of cooperation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ullmann-Margalit, E. (1977). The emergence of norms. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Zagzebski, L. (1996). Virtues of the mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adey, P. & Shayer, M. (2002). Cognitive acceleration comes of age. In Shayer, M. & Adey, P. (eds.), Learning intelligence. Buckingham: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Aristotle (1987). On the soul. In Ackrill, J. (ed.), New Aristotle reader. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Audi, R. (1999). The Cambridge dictionary of philosophy. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bell, V. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1998). A model theory of modal reasoning. Cognitive Science, 22, 25–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bereiter, C. (2002). Education and mind in the knowledge age.Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. (2003). The biological emergence of representation. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. & Pellegrini, A. (2002). The origins of human nature.Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Braine, M. & O'Brien, D. (1998). Mental logic. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brandom, R. (1994). Making it explicit: reasoning, representing, and discursive commitment.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brandom, R. (2000). Articulating reasons: an introduction to inferentialism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bryant, P. (2001). Learning in Geneva: the contribution of Bärbel Inhelder and her colleagues. In Typhon, A. & Vonèche, J. (eds.), Working with Piaget: essays in honour of Bärbel Inhelder. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. & Duff, M. (1989). Young children's comprehension of modal expressions. Cognitive Development, 4, 369–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carl, W. (1994). Frege's theory of sense and reference: its origins and scope. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, L. (1895). What the tortoise said to Achilles. Mind, 4, 278–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D. (1996). The conscious mind: in search of a fundamental theory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Changeux, J-P. (2000). Co-author. In Changeux, J-P. & Ricoeur, P., What makes us think?Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cheng, P. & Holyoak, K. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (2004). What is evolutionary psychology? Explaining the new science of the mind. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Cresswell, M. & Hughes, G. (1996). A new introduction to modal logic. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1999). The extended phenotype: the long reach of the gene. Revised edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1931). Meditations on first philosophy. In Haldane, E. & Ross, G. (eds.), Philosophical works of Descartes, vol. I. New York: Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Dummett, M. (1981). Frege: philosophy of language. 2nd edition. London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
Evans, J. (2002). Logic and human reasoning: an assessment of the deduction paradigm. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 978–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, M., Pinard, A. & Runions, K. (2001). Piaget's framework for a scientific study of consciousness. Human Development, 44, 195–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2003). Is there a faculty of deontic reasoning? A critical re-evaluation of abstract deontic versions of the Wason selection task. In Over, D. (ed.). Evolution and the psychology of thinking. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2004). Domains of deontic reasoning: resolving the discrepancy between the cognitive and moral reasoning literatures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 447–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J., Miller, P. & Miller, S. (1993). Cognitive development. 3rd edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1950). The foundations of arithmetic. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1964). The basic laws of arithmetic. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1977). Logical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1979). Posthumous papers. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (2001). Pathways of knowledge: public and private. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gruber, H. & Vonèche, J. (1995). The essential Piaget. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson Inc.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. & Núñez, M. (1996). Understanding of permission rules by preschool children. Child Development, 67, 1572–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawking, S. (2001). The universe in a nutshell. London: Bantam Press.Google Scholar
Holbo, J. (2002). Moral dilemma and the logic of obligation. American Philosophical Quarterly, 39, 259–74.Google Scholar
Horty, J. F. (2001). Agency and deontic logic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, G. (1951). Thinking. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1868). A liberal education. In Bibby, C. (1971), Huxley on education. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Isaacs, N. (1951). Critical notice. British Journal of Psychology, 42, 185–8.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. (1999). Deductive reasoning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 109–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kant, I. (1993). Critique of practical reason. 3rd edition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1997). Lectures on ethics. In Collected works. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, A. (1995). Frege. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1992). The naturalists return. The Philosophical Review, 101, 53–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klahr, D. (1999). The conceptual habitat: in what kind of system can concepts develop? In Scholnick, E., Nelson, K., Gelman, S. & Miller, P. (eds.), Conceptual development: Piaget's legacy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Korsgaard, C. (1996). The sources of normativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kripke, S. (1982). Wittgenstein on rules and private language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1977). The essential tension. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kusch, M. (1995). Psychologism: a case study in the sociology of philosophical knowledge. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lakatos, I. (1974). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In Lakatos, I. & Musgrave, A. (eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1994). The logical foundations of cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mason, H. E. (1996). Moral dilemmas and moral theory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Morf, A. (1957). Les relations entre la logique et la langage lors du passage du raisonnement concrèt au raisonnement formel. In Apostel, L., Mandelbrot, B. & Morf, A., Logique, langage, et théorie de l'information. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Morgan, P. (2005). The insider. London: Ebury Press.Google Scholar
Moser, P. (1999). Epistemology. In Audi, R. (ed.), The Cambridge dictionary of philosophy. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1995). Other minds: critical essays 1969–94. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1918). Recherche. Lausanne: La Concorde.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1950). Introduction à l'épistémologie génétique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, vol. I.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The child's conception of number. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1953). Logic and psychology. Manchester: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1965). Discussion: genèse et structure en psychologie. In Gandillac, M. & Goldman, L. (eds.), Entretiens sur les notions de genèse et de structure. Paris: Mouton & Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1966). Part II. In Beth, E. & Piaget, J., Mathematical epistemology and psychology. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970a). Genetic epistemology. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970b). A conversation with Jean Piaget. Psychology Today, 3, 25–32.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1973). Main trends in psychology. London: George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1974). The child and reality. London: Frederick Muller Ltd.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1985). Equilibration of cognitive structures. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1986). Essay on necessity. Human Development, 29, 301–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in reflecting abstraction. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (2006). Reason. New Ideas in Psychology.CrossRef
Piaget, J. & Garcia, J. (1991). Toward a logic of meanings. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1979). Objective knowledge. 2nd edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Priest, G. (1998). What is so bad about contradictions?The Journal of Philosophy, 95, 410–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (2002). The collapse of the fact/value dichotomy, and other essays. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Quine, W. (1972). Methods of logic. 3rd edition. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, P. (2000). Co-author. In Changeux, J-P. & Ricoeur, P., What makes us think?Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ross, A. (1968). Directives and norms. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Sainsbury, R. M. (1991). Logical forms. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Scholnick, E. & Wing, C. (1995). Logic in conversation: comparative studies of deduction in children and adults. Cognitive Development, 10, 319–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1999). Mind, language and society. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (2001). Rationality in action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. (1997). Piaget and Vygotsky: a necessary marriage for effective intervention. In Smith, L., Dockrell, J. & Tomlinson, P. (eds.), Piaget, Vygotsky and beyond. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. & Adey, P. (1981). Towards a science of science teaching. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. & Adey, P. (2002). Learning intelligence. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. & Adhami, M. (2003). Realising the cognitive potential of children 5–7 with a mathematics focus. International Journal of Educational Research, 39, 743–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sluga, H. (1980). Gottlob Frege. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1993). Necessary knowledge. Hove, UK: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1998). On the development of mental representation. Developmental Review, 18, 202–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (1999a). What Piaget learned from Frege. Developmental Review, 19, 133–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (1999b). Epistemological principles for developmental psychology in Frege and Piaget. New Ideas in Psychology, 17, 83–117, 137–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (1999c). Representation and knowledge are not the same thing. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 22, 784–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (2002a). Reasoning by mathematical induction in children's arithmetic. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2002b). Piaget's model. In Goswami, U. (ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (2003). From epistemology to psychology in the development of knowledge. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2004). Developmental epistemology and education. In Carpendale, J. & Müller, U. (eds.), Social interaction and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wason, P. (1966). Reasoning. In Foss, B. (ed.), New horizons in psychology. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin.Google Scholar
Wason, P. (1977). The theory of formal operations: a critique. In Gerber, B. (ed.), Piaget and knowing. LondonRoutledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical investigations. 2nd edition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1978). Remarks on the foundations of mathematics. 3rd edition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1963). Norm and action. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1983a). Practical reason. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1983b). Truth, knowledge, and modality. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1985). The tree of knowledge. Leiden: E. J. Brill.Google Scholar
Boesch, E. E. (1991). Symbolic action theory and cultural psychology. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breit, H. & Eckensberger, L. H. (2004). Die Faktizität des Normenbewusstseins: eine entwicklungs psychologische Perspektive. In Lütge, C. & Vollmer, G. (eds.), Fakten statt Normen? Zur Rolle einzelwissenschaftlicher Argumente in einer naturalistischen Ethik. Baden-Baden: Nomos-Verlag, pp. 207–24.Google Scholar
Cole, M. (1998). Cultural psychology: a once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dasen, P. R. & Heron, A. (1981). Cross-cultural tests of Piaget's theory. In Triandis, H. C. & Heron, A. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, vol. IV: Developmental psychology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, pp. 295–342.Google Scholar
Day, J. & Tappan, M. (1996). The narrative approach to moral development: from the epistemic subject to dialogical selves. Human Development, 32, 67–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1986). Handlung, Konflikt und Reflexion: zur Dialektik von Struktur und Inhalt im moralischen Urteil. In Edelstein, W. & Nunner-Winkler, G. (eds.), Zur Bestimmung der Moral: philosophische und sozialwissenschaftliche Beiträge zur Moralforschung. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, pp. 409–42.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1990). From cross-cultural psychology to cultural psychology. The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 12 (1), 37–52.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1993). Normative und deskriptive, strukturelle und empirische Anteile in moralischen Urteilen: ein Ökonomie-Ökologie Konflikt aus psychologischer Sicht. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Gähde, U. (eds.), Ethische Norm und empirische Hypothese. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp Verlag, pp. 328–79.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1995). Activity or action: two different roads towards an integration of culture into psychology?Culture & Psychology, 1, 67–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1996). Agency, action and culture: three basic concepts for cross-cultural psychology. In Pandey, J., Sinha, D. & Bhawuk, D. P. S. (eds.), Asian contributions to cross-cultural psychology. New Delhi: Sage Publications, pp. 72–102.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (2003). Wanted: a contextualized psychology: a plea for cultural psychology based on actual psychology. In Saraswathi, T. S. (ed.), Cross-cultural perspectives in human development: theory, research, and application. New Delhi: Sage Publications, pp. 70–101.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. & Emminghaus, W. B. (1982). Moralisches Urteil und Aggression: zur Systematisierung und Präzisierung des Aggressionskonzeptes sowie einiger empirischer Befunde. In Hilke, R. & Kempf, W. (eds.), Aggression: naturwissenschaftliche und kulturwissenschaftliche Perspektiven der Aggressionsforschung. Bern: Huber, pp. 208–80.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. & Meacham, J. (1984). The essentials of action theory: a framework for discussion. Human Development, 27, 166–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Eckensberger, L. H. & Plath, I. (in press). Soziale Kognitionen. In Schneider, W. & Sodian, B. (eds.), Enzyklopädie für Psychologie, Kognitive Entwicklung, C Theorie und Forschung, Serie V Entwicklung, vol. II. Göttingen: Hogrefe, pp. 407–91.
Eckensberger, L. H. & Reinshagen, H. (1980). Kohlbergs Stufentheorie der Entwicklung des moralischen Urteils: ein Versuch ihrer Reinterpretation im Bezugsrahmen handlungstheoretischer Konzepte. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Silbereisen, R. K. (eds.), Entwicklung sozialer Kognitionen: Modelle, Theorien, Methoden, Anwendung. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta, pp. 65–131.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. & Zimba, R. F. (1997). The development of moral judgment. In Dasen, P. & Saraswathi, T. S. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology. 2nd edition, vol. II. Developmental psychology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, pp. 299–338.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H., Breit, H. & Döring, T. (1996). Moral judgments in the context of ecological and economical value orientations: the case of a coal-fuelled power station. Paper read at the 22nd Annual Conference of the Association for Moral Education (AME) Ottawa, November, 1996.
Eckensberger, L. H., Breit, H. & Döring, T. (1997). Moral judgments and knowledge in the context of ecological and economical value orientations: the case of a coal-fuelled power plant. Paper read at the 23rd Annual Conference of the Association for Moral Education (AME) Atlanta, November, 1997.
Eckensberger, L. H., Breit, H. & Döring, T. (1999a). Ethik und Barrieren in umweltbezogenen Entscheidungen: eine entwicklungspsychologische Perspektive. In Linneweber, V. & Kals, E. (eds.), Umweltgerechtes Handeln. Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 165–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H., Breit, H.& Döring, T. (1999b). The role of affects in contextualized moral judgments. Paper read at the 25th Annual Conference of the Association for Moral Education (AME) Minneapolis, November, 1999.
Eckensberger, L. H., Döring, T. & Breit, H. (2001). Moral dimensions in risk evaluation. Research in Social Problems and Public Policy, 9, 137–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelhardt, H. D. (1975). Umweltstrategie: Materialien und Analysen zu einer Umweltethik in der Industriegesellschaft. Gütersloh: Mohn.Google Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life-cycle. New York: International University Press.Google Scholar
Fowler, J. W. (1981). Stages of faith. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1998). Surrogates for theories. Theory & Psychology, 8 (2), 195–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haan, N. (1977). Coping and defending: processes of self-environment organization. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Habermas, J. (1981). Theorie des kommunikativen Handelns, vol. I: Handlungsrationalität und geschlechtliche Rationalisierung. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1966). Groundwork of the metaphysics of morals. In Paton, H. (ed.), The moral law. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Kasper, E., Sieloff, U., Nieder, A. & Eckensberger, L. H. (1987). Entwicklung eines Tests zur Erfassung des Wissens über die Kraftwerksproblematik. Arbeiten der Fachrichtung Psychologie, Universität des Saarlandes, 114.
Kegan, R. (1982). The emerging self. Harvard: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kesselring, T. (1981). Entwicklung und Widerspruch: ein Vergleich zwischen Piagets genetischer Erkenntnistheorie und Hegels Dialektik. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1973). The claim to moral adequacy of a highest stage of moral judgment. Journal of Philosophy, 70, 630–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. & Launier, R. (1978). Stress-related transactions between person and environment. In Pervin, L. A. & Lewis, M. (eds.), Perspective in interactional psychology. New York: Plenum, pp. 287–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leontiev, A. N. (1977). Tätigkeit, Bewusstsein, Persönlichkeit (Activity, consciousness, personality). Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Lourenço, O. (1996). Reflections on narrative approaches to moral development. Human Development, 39, 83–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meggle, G. (1993). Semantische, empirische und normative Aspekte eines Begriffs des kommunikativen Handelns. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Gähde, U. (eds.), Ethische Norm und empirische Hypothese. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, pp. 197–221.
Oser, F. & Gmünder, P. (1984). Der Mensch: Stufen seiner religiösen Entwicklung. Zürich: Benziger.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1918). Recherche. Lausanne: La Concorde.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.), Carmichael's handbook of child psychology, vol. I. New York: Wiley, pp. 703–32.
Piaget, J. (1977). The grasp of consciousness. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul ((1974). La prise de conscience. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France).
Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: their relationship during child development, trans. and ed. Brown, T. A. & Kaegi, C. E.. Palo Alto, California: Annual Reviews Inc.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies. London: Routledge ((1965). Etudes sociologiques. Geneva: Droz).
Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in reflecting abstraction. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Rosnow, R. L. & Georgoudi, M. (1986). The spirit of contextualism. In Rosnow, R. L. & Georgoudi, M. (eds.), Contextualism and understanding in behavioral science. New York: Praeger, pp. 3–22.Google Scholar
Schwartz, L. (1951). Die Neurosen und die dynamische Psychologie von Pierre Janet. Basel, Switzerland: Schwabe.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1990). Cultural psychology – what is it? In Stigler, J. W., Shweder, R. A. & Herdt, G. (eds.), Cultural psychology: essays on comparative human development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sieloff, U., Schirk, S., Kasper, E., Nieder, A. & Eckensberger, L. H. (1988). Eine textanalytische Auswertungstechnik zur Erfassung von Abwehr und Bewältigung beim moralischen Argumentieren. Arbeiten der Fachrichtung Psychologie, 118, Saarbrücken: Universität des Saarlandes.
Valsiner, J. (1987). Culture and the development of children's action. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1972). Tractatus logico-philosophicus. 2nd edition. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1971). Explanation and understanding. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Arsenio, W. F. & Lover, A. (1995). Children's conceptions of sociomoral affect: happy victimizers, mixed emotions, and other expectancies. In Killen, M. & Hart, D. (eds.), Morality in everyday life. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 87–128.Google Scholar
Barrett, C. D., Keller, M., Takezawa, M. & Wichary, S. (submitted). Children's understanding of social emotions: a study in ecological rationality.
Berndt, T. J. (1993). The morality of friendship versus the morality of individual autonomy. Paper presented at the symposium ‘Spheres of Morality’ at the 60th anniversary meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, March, 1993.
Bersoff, D. M. & Miller, J. G. (1993). Culture, context and the development of moral accountability judgments. Developmental Psychology, 29, 664–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: a critical review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 1–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blasi, A. (2004). Moral functioning: moral understanding and personality. In Lapsley & Narváez (eds.), pp. 335–48.
Blum, L. A. (1980). Friendship, altruism and morality. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Bond, M. H. (ed.) (1996). The handbook of Chinese psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boyes, M. & Walker, L. J. (1988). Implications of cultural diversity for the universality claims of Kohlberg's theory of moral reasoning. Human Development, 31, 44–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bukowski, W. M. & Sippola, L. K. (1996). Friendship and morality: how are they related? In Bukowski, W. M., Newcomb, A. F. & Hartup, W. W. (eds.), The company they keep. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 238–61.Google Scholar
Colby, A. & Kohlberg, L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgment, vol. I. Theoretical foundations and research validation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (eds.), The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 163–228.Google Scholar
Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Döbert, R. (1989). Moralische Erziehung in der Volksrepublik China (Moral education in the People's Republic of China). Vergleichende Pädagogik, 25, 246–59.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. & Zimba, R. F. (1997). The development of moral judgment. In Dasen, P. R. & Saraswathi, T. S. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, vol. III. Developmental psychology. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, pp. 299–338.Google Scholar
Edelstein, W. & Noam, G. G. (1982). Regulatory structures of the self and ‘postformal’ stages in adulthood. Human Development, 25, 407–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (1982). The development of reasoning regarding prosocial behavior. In Eisenberg, N. (ed.), The development of prosocial behavior. New York: Academic Press, pp. 219–49.Google Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2004). Domains of deontic reasoning: resolving the discrepancy between the cognitive and moral reasoning literatures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 447–74.CrossRef
Frankena, W. K. (1973). Ethics. 2nd edition. London: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Gerson, R. R. & Damon, W. (1978). Moral understanding and children's conduct. In Damon, W. (ed.), Moral development. New directions for child development, No. 2. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, pp. 41–61.Google Scholar
Gibbard, A. (1992). Wise choices, apt feelings: a theory of normative judgement. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, J. C. & Widaman, K. F. (1982). Social intelligence: measuring the development of sociomoral reflection. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, R. & So-Kum Tang, C. (1996). Chinese personal relationships. In Bond (ed.), pp. 294–308.
Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: a preliminary statement. American Sociological Review, 25, 161–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: implications for caring and justice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M. (1984). Resolving conflicts in friendship: the development of moral understanding in everyday life. In Kurtines, W. M. & Gewirtz, J. L. (eds.), Morality, moral behaviour and moral development. New York: Wiley, pp. 140–58.Google Scholar
Keller, M. (1996). Moralische Sensibilität: Entwicklung in Freundschaft und Familie (Moral sensibility: development in friendship and family). Weinheim: Psychologie-Verlags Union.Google Scholar
Keller, M., in cooperation with Cecora, L., Eckert, U., Fang, Fu-xi & Fang, Ge (2004a). Self in relationship. In Lapsley & Narváez (eds.), pp. 267–98.
Keller, M. & Edelstein, W. (1990). The emergence of morality in personal relationships. In Wren, T. E. (ed.), The moral domain: essays in the ongoing discussion between philosophy and the social sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 255–82.Google Scholar
Keller, M. & Edelstein, W. (1991). The development of socio-moral meaning making: domains, categories and perspective-taking. In Kurtines, W. M. & Gewirtz, J. L. (eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development, vol. II. Research. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 89–114.Google Scholar
Keller, M. & Edelstein, W. (1993). The development of the moral self from childhood to adolescence. In Noam & Wren (eds.), pp. 310–36.
Keller, M. & Reuss, S. (1984). An action-theoretical reconstruction of the development of social-cognitive competence. Human Development, 27, 211–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M., Eckensberger, L. H. & Rosen, K. (1989). A critical note on the conception of preconventional morality: the case of stage 2 in Kohlberg's theory. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 57–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M., Gummerum, M., Wang, X. T. & Lindsey, S. (2004b). Understanding perspectives and emotions in contract violation: development of deontic and moral reasoning. Child Development, 75, 614–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M., Lourenço, O., Malti, T. & Saalbach, H. (2003). The multifaceted phenomenon of ‘happy victimizers’: a cross-cultural comparison. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M., Edelstein, W., Fang, Fu-xi, Hong, Tang & Schuster, P. (1996). The role of culture in the attribution of moral feelings. Paper presented at the 23rd Annual Conference of the Association for Moral Education, Ottawa, November, 1996.
Keller, M., Edelstein, W., Krettenauer, T., Fang, F. & Fang, G. (2005). Reasoning about moral obligations and interpersonal responsibilities in different cultural contexts. In W. Edelstein & G. Nunner-Winkler (eds.), Morality in context. Advances in Psychology 137. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 317–40.CrossRef
Keller, M., Edelstein, W., Schmid, C., Fang, F. & Fang, G. (1998). Reasoning about responsibilities and obligations in close relationships: a comparison across two cultures. Developmental Psychology, 34, 731–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohlberg, L. (1976). Moral stages and moralization: the cognitive developmental approach. In Lickona, T. (ed.), Moral development and behavior: theory, research and social issues. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, pp. 31–53.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development, vol. II. The psychology of moral development: the nature and validity of moral stages. San Francisco: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Krappmann, L. (1996). Amicitia, drujba, shin-yu, philia, Freundschaft, friendship: on the cultural diversity of a human relationship. In Bukowski, W. M., Newcomb, A. F. & Hartup, W. W. (eds.), The company they keep: friendship in childhood and adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 19–40.Google Scholar
Krettenauer, T. (2004). Metaethical cognition and epistemic reasoning development in adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 68, 461–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krettenauer, T. & Eichler, D. (2005). Adolescents' self-attributed emotions following a moral transgression: relations with delinquency, confidence in moral judgment, and age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology.
Lapsley, D. K. & Narváez, D. (eds.) (2004). Moral development, self and identity. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ma, H. K. (1988). The Chinese perspectives on moral judgment development. International Journal of Psychology, 23, 201–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisan, M. (2004). Judgement and choice in moral functioning. In Lapsley & Narváez (eds.), pp. 133–60.
Noam, G. G. & Wren, T. E. (1993) in cooperation with Nunner-Winkler, G. & Edelstein, W. (eds.), The moral self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Nucci, L. P. & Lee, J. (1993). Morality and personal autonomy. In Noam & Wren (eds.), pp. 123–48.
Nunner-Winkler, G. (1993). The growth of moral motivation. In Noam & Wren (eds.), pp. 269–91.
Nunner-Winkler, G. & Sodian, B. (1988). Children's understanding of moral emotions. Child Development, 59, 323–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oser, F. & Reichenbach, R. (2005). Moral resilience – the unhappy moralist. In W. Edelstein & G. Nunner-Winkler (eds.), Morality in context. Advances in Psychology 137. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 203–24.CrossRef
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Free Press. (Original work published 1932.)Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Richards, D. A. J. (1971). A theory of reasons for action. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding: developmental and clinical analyses. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M. & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and moral development. In Kagan, J. and Lamb, S. (eds.), The emergence of morality in young children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 1–83.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. G. (1982). Concepts of self and morality. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and collectivism. In Berman, J. J. (ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1989, vol. XXXVII. Cross-cultural perspectives. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, pp. 41–133.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). Domains and categories in social cognitive development. In Overton, W. F. (ed.), The relationship between social and cognitive development. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 53–90.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In Damon, W. (series ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (vol. ed.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. III. Social, emotional and personality development. 5th edition. New York: Wiley, pp. 863–932.Google Scholar
Youniss, J. (1980). Parents and peers in social development. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Abu-Lughod, L. (1993). Writing women's worlds: Bedouin stories. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Antipoff, H. (1928). Observations sur la compassion et le sens de la justice chez l'enfant. Archives de Psychologie, 21.Google Scholar
Asch, S. E. (1952). Social psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, R. (1934). Patterns of culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Bok, S. (1978/1999). Lying: moral choice in public and private life. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Clémence, A., Doise, W., Rosa, A. S. & Gonzalez, L. (1995). La représentation sociale des droits de l'homme: une recherche internationale sur l'étendue et les limites de l'universalité. Journal Internationale de Psychologie, 30, 181–212.Google Scholar
Davidson, P., Turiel, E. & Black, A. (1983). The effect of stimulus familiarity on the use of criteria and justifications in children's social reasoning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 49–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doise, W., Clémence, A. & Spini, D. (1996). Human rights and social psychology. The British Psychological Society Social Psychology Section Newsletter, 35, 3–21.Google Scholar
Dworkin, R. (1977). Taking rights seriously. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2004). Domains of deontic reasoning: resolving the discrepancy between the cognitive and moral reasoning literatures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 447–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2005). Domains of deontic reasoning: a postscript. Paper presented at the 35th Annual Meeting of the Jean Piaget Society. Vancouver, BC, Canada, June 2005.
Freeman, V. G., Rathore, S. S., Weinfurt, K. P., Schulman, K. A. & Sulmasy, D. P. (1999). Lying for patients: physician deception of third-party payers. Archives of Internal Medicine, 159, 2,263–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gewirth, A. (1982). Human rights: essays on justification and applications. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatch, E. (1983). Culture and morality: the relativity of values in anthropology. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Helwig, C. C. (1995). Adolescents' and young adults' conceptions of civil liberties: freedom of speech and religion. Child Development, 66, 152–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helwig, C. C. (1997). The role of agent and social context in judgments of freedom of speech and religion. Child Development, 68, 484–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyman, H. H. & Sheatsley, P. B. (1953). Trends in public opinion on civil liberties. Journal of Social Issues, 9, 6–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, M. L. Jr. (1968). The role of the behavioral scientist in the civil rights movement. American Psychologist, 23, 180–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: the cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In Goslin, D. (ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally, pp. 347–480.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1971). From is to ought: how to commit the naturalistic fallacy and get away with it in the study of moral development. In Mischel, T. (ed.), Psychology and genetic epistemology. New York: Academic Press, pp. 151–235.Google Scholar
McClosky, M. & Brill, A. (1983). Dimensions of tolerance: what Americans believe about civil liberties. New York: Russell Sage.Google Scholar
Nucci, L. P. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L. P. & Nucci, M. S. (1982a). Children's responses to moral and social conventional transgressions in free-play settings. Child Development, 53, 1,337–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L. P. & Nucci, M. S. (1982b). Children's social interactions in the context of moral and conventional transgressions. Child Development, 53, 403–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perkins, S. A. (2003). Adolescent reasoning about lying in close relationships. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of California, Berkeley.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1951/1995). Egocentric thought and sociocentric thought. In Piaget, J., Sociological studies. London: Routledge, pp. 270–86.Google Scholar
Pizarro, D. A. & Bloom, P. (2003). The intelligence of the moral intuitions: comment on Haidt (2001). Psychological Review, 110, 193–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Protho, J. W. & Grigg, C. M. (1960). Fundamental principles of democracy: bases of agreement and disagreement. Journal of Politics, 22, 276–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarat, A. (1975). Reasoning in politics: the social, political, and psychological bases of principled thought. American Journal of Political Science, 19, 247–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smetana, J. G. (1995). Morality in context: abstractions, ambiguities, and applications. In Vasta, R. (ed.), Annals of child development, vol. X. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers, pp. 83–130.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. G. (2002). Culture, autonomy, and personal jurisdiction in adolescent–parent relationships. In Reese, H. W. & Kail, R. (eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, vol. XXIX. New York: Academic Press, pp. 51–87.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2003). From epistemology to psychology in the development of knowledge. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 201–28.Google Scholar
Stouffer, S. (1955). Communism, conformity and civil liberties. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Tisak, M. S. & Turiel, E. (1984). Children's conceptions of moral and prudential rules. Child Development, 55, 1,030–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: morality and convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In W. Damon (ed.), Handbook of child psychology. 5th edition, vol. III: N. Eisenberg (ed.), Social, emotional, and personality development. New York: Wiley, pp. 863–932.
Turiel, E. (2002). The culture of morality: social development, context, and conflict. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. (2003). Resistance and subversion in everyday life. Journal of Moral Education, 32, 115–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. (2005). Thought about actions in social domains: morality, social conventions, and social interactions. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley.
Turiel, E. (2006). Thought, emotions, and social interactional processes in moral development. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. G. (eds.), Handbook of moral development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 7–35.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. & Perkins, S. A. (2004). Flexibilities of mind: conflict and culture. Human Development, 47, 158–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. & Wainryb, C. (1998). Concepts of freedoms and rights in a traditional hierarchically organized society. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 375–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E., Perkins, S. A. & Mensing, J. F. (in preparation). Judgments about deception in marital relationships. University of California, Berkeley.
Wason, P. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 273–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wertheimer, M. (1935). Some problems in the theory of ethics. Social Research, 2, 353–67.Google Scholar
Wikan, U. (1996). Tomorrow, God willing: self-made destinies in Cairo. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wynia, M. K., Cummins, D. S., VanGeest, J. B. & Wilson, I. B. (2000). Physician manipulation of reimbursement rules for patients: between a rock and a hard place. Journal of the American Medical Association, 283, 1,858–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D. (2005). Measuring the mind: conceptual issues in contemporary psychometrics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erneling, C. E. & Johnson, D. M. (eds.) (2005). The mind as a scientific object: between brain and culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Habermas, J. (1987). Theorie des kommunikativen Handelns, vol. I. Handlungsrationalität und gesellschaftliche Rationalisierung, vol. II. Zur Kritik der funktionalistischen Vernunft. 4th edition. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.
Heidelberger, M. (2004). Nature from within: Gustav T. Fechner and his psychophysical worldview. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Kusch, M. (1995). Psychologism: a case study in the sociology of philosophical knowledge. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lord, F. M. & Novick, M. R. (1968). Statistical theories of mental test scores. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
McCarthy, G. E. (2001). Objectivity and the silence of reason: Weber, Habermas, and the methodological disputes in German sociology. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Molenaar, P. C. M. (2003). State space techniques in structural equation modeling: transformation of latent variables in and out of latent variable models. http://www.hhdev.psu.edu/hdfs/faculty/molenaar.html
Molenaar, P. C. M. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2, 201–18.Google Scholar
Nesselroade, J. R. & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2003). Quantitative models for developmental processes. In Valsiner, J. & Connolly, K. (eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology. London: Sage, pp. 622–39.Google Scholar
Oddie, G. (2005). Value, reality, and desire. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (2004). The collapse of the fact/value dichotomy and other essays. 3rd edition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Acredolo, C., O'Connor, J., Banks, L. & Horobin, K. (1989). Children's ability to make probability estimates: skills revealed through application of Anderson's functional measurement methodology. Child Development, 60, 933–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozzi, P. (1990). Fisica ingenua (Naive physics). Milan: Garzanti.Google Scholar
Brainerd, C. J. (1981). Working memory and the developmental analysis of probability judgment. Psychological Review, 88, 463–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition, 58, 1–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
da Buti, F. (1852–62). Commento di Francesco da Buti sopra La Divina Commedia di Dante Allighieri (Francesco da Buti's commentary on Dante's Divine Comedy). Pisa: Nistri. (Originally written circa 1385.) Available at http://dciswww.dartmouth.eduGoogle Scholar
David, F. N. (1962). Games, gods and gambling: a history of probability and statistical ideas. London: Griffin.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: how the mind creates mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
della Lana, J. (1866–7). Comedia di Dante degli Allaghieri col commento di Jacopo della Lana bolognese (Dante's Comedy, commentary by Jacopo della Lana from Bologna). Bologna: Tipografia Regia. (Originally written circa 1324.) Available at http://dciswww.dartmouth.eduGoogle Scholar
Fischbein, E. (1975). The intuitive sources of probabilistic thinking in children. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, J. (2001). The science of conjecture: evidence and probability before Pascal. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1993). The bounded rationality of probabilistic mental models. In Manktelow, K. & Over, D. (eds.), Rationality. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve bayesian reasoning without instruction: frequency format. Psychological Review, 102, 684–704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. & Kahneman, D. (eds.) (2002). Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girotto, V. (2004). Task understanding. In Leighton, J. & Sternberg, R. (eds.), The nature of reasoning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (2001). Solving probabilistic and statistical problems: a matter of information structure and question form. Cognition, 78, 247–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (2002). Chances and frequencies in probabilistic reasoning: rejoinder to Hoffrage, Gigerenzer, Krauss, and Martignon. Cognition, 84, 353–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (2003). Early reasoning about chances and possibilities: historical and developmental evidence. Paper presented at the 16th Jean Piaget Archives Conference, University of Geneva, September.
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (2005). Probabilistic reasoning and combinatorial analysis. In Girotto, V. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (eds.), The shape of reason. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (in press). Extensional reasoning about chances. In Schaeken, W., Vooght, G., Vandierendonck, A. & d'Ydewalle, G. (eds.), Mental model theory: extensions and refinements. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hacking, I. (1975). The emergence of probability. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hilton, D. (1995). The social context of reasoning: conversational inference and rational judgement. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 248–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howson, C. & Urbach, P. (1993). Scientific reasoning: the Bayesian approach. Chicago: Open Court.Google Scholar
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1964). The early growth of logic. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1955.)Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Legrenzi, P., Girotto, V., Sonino-Legrenzi, M. & Caverni, J. P. (1999). Naive probability: a model theory of extensional reasoning. Psychological Review, 106, 62–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (eds.) (2000). Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kendall, M. G. (1956). The beginnings of a probability calculus. Biometrika, 43, 1–14.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1975). An essay concerning human understanding. Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Original work published 1690.)Google Scholar
Neyman, J. & Pearson, E. S. (1967). Joint statistical papers of J. Neyman and E. S. Pearson. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Krantz, D. H., Jepson, C. & Kunda, D. (1983). The use of statistical heuristics in everyday inductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 90, 339–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ore, O. (1965). Cardano, the gambling scholar. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Ottimo, A. L. (1827–9). L'Ottimo commento della Divina Commedia (Ottimo's commentary on the Divine Comedy). Pisa: Capurro. (Originally written circa 1331.) Available at http://dciswww.dartmouth.eduGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1975). The origin of the idea of chance in children. New York: Norton. (Original work published 1951.)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Szeminska, B. (1952). The child's conception of number. New York: Humanities Press. (Original work published 1941.)Google Scholar
Politzer, G. & Noveck, I. A. (1991). Are conjunction rule violations the results of conversational rule violations?Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 83–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, L. J. (1954). The foundations of statistics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sloman, S. A., Over, D., Slovak, L. & Stibel, J. M. (2003). Frequency illusions and other fallacies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 296–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tentori, K., Bonini, N. & Osherson, D. (2004). The conjunction fallacy: a misunderstanding about conjunction?Cognitive Science, 26, 467–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, 293–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yost, P. A., Siegel, A. E. & Andrews, J. M. (1962). Nonverbal probability judgments by young children. Child Development, 33, 769–80.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhu, L. & Gigerenzer, G. (2006). Children can solve Bayesian problems: the role of representation in mental computation. Cognition, 98, 287–308.CrossRef
Barrouillet, P. & Lecas, J. F. (1999). Mental models in conditional reasoning and working memory. Thinking and Reasoning, 5 (4), 289–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrouillet, P., Markovits, H. & Quinn, S. (2002). Developmental and content effects in reasoning with causal conditionals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 235–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D. F. & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10, 48–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1995). The evolution of inhibition mechanisms and their role in human cognition and behavior. In Dempster, F. N. & Brainerd, C. J. (eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition. London: Academic Press, pp. 141–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1990). The ‘natural logic’ approach to reasoning. In Overton, W. F. (ed.), Reasoning, necessity and logic: developmental perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc., pp. 133–57.Google Scholar
Bucci, W. (1978). The interpretation of universal affirmative propositions. Cognition, 6, 55–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cahan, S. & Artman, L. (1997). Is everyday experience dysfunctional for the development of conditional reasoning?Cognitive Development, 12 (2), 261–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, D. D. (1995). Naive theories and causal deduction. Memory and Cognition, 23 (5), 646–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummins, D. D., Lubart, T., Alksnis, O. & Rist, R. (1991). Conditional reasoning and causation. Memory and Cognition, 19 (3), 274–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, R. (1999). The extended phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
De, Neys W., Schaeken, W. & d'Ydewalle, G. (2002). Causal conditional reasoning and semantic memory retrieval: a test of the ‘semantic memory framework’. Memory and Cognition, 30 (6), 908–20.Google Scholar
Dias, M. G. & Harris, P. L. (1988). The effect of make-believe play on deductive reasoning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 207–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dias, M. G. & Harris, P. L. (1990). The influence of the imagination on reasoning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 305–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J.St., B. T. & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Evans, J.St., B. T. & Over, D. E. (2004). If. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103 (4), 650–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, J., Pea, R. D., Glick, J. & Scribner, S. (1984). ‘Merds that laugh don't like mushrooms’: evidence for deductive reasoning by preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 20 (4), 584–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janveau-Brennan, G. & Markovits, H. (1999). The development of reasoning with causal conditionals. Developmental Psychology, 35 (4), 904–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Byrne, R. M. J. (1991). Deduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Byrne, R. M. J. (2002). Conditionals: a theory of meaning, pragmatics, and inference. Psychological Review, 109 (4), 646–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1996). On the reality of cognitive illusions. Psychological Review, 103 (3), 582–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, R. (1992). Processing speed, speech rate, and memory. Developmental Psychology, 28, 899–904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaczynski, P. A. & Narashimham, G. (1998). Representations as mediators of adolescent deductive reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 5, 865–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kokis, J. V., Macpherson, R., Toplak, M. E., West, R. F. & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Heuristic and analytic processing: age trends and associations with cognitive ability and cognitive styles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83 (1), 26–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lea, R. B., O'Brien, D. P., Fisch, S., Braine, M. D. S. & Noveck, I. (1990). Predicting propositional logic inferences in text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 361–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leevers, H. & Harris, P. (1999). Transient and persisting effects of instruction on young children's syllogistic reasoning with incongruent and abstract premises. Thinking and Reasoning, 5 (2), 145–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. (1985). Incorrect conditional reasoning among adults: competence or performance?British Journal of Psychology, 76, 241–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. (1995). Conditional reasoning with false premises: fantasy and information retrieval. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. (2000). A mental model analysis of young children's conditional reasoning with meaningful premises. Thinking and Reasoning, 6 (4), 335–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. & Barrouillet, P. (2002). The development of conditional reasoning: a mental model account. Developmental Review, 22 (1), 5–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. & Doyon, C. (2004). Information processing and reasoning with premises that are not empirically true: interference, working memory and processing speed. Memory and Cognition, 32 (4), 592–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. & Handley, S. (in press). Is inferential reasoning just probabilistic reasoning in disguise? Memory and Cognition.
Markovits, H. & Quinn, S. (2002). Efficiency of retrieval correlates with ‘logical’ reasoning from causal conditional premises. Memory and Cognition, 30 (5), 696–706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. & Vachon, R. (1989). Reasoning with contrary-to-fact propositions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 398–412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markovits, H. & Vachon, R. (1990). Conditional reasoning, representation and level of abstraction. Developmental Psychology, 26, 942–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H., Venet, M., Janveau-Brennan, G., Malfait, N., Pion, N. & Vadeboncoeur, I. (1996). Reasoning in young children: fantasy and information retrieval. Child Development, 67, 2,857–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neil, W. T. (1997). Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 23 (6), 1,291–3,105.Google Scholar
Oaksford, M., Chater, N. & Larkin, J. (2000). Probabilities and polarity biases in conditional inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 26 (4), 883–99.Google ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, D. P. & Overton, W. F. (1980). Conditional reasoning following contradictory evidence: a developmental analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 44–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, D. P. & Overton, W. F. (1982). Conditional reasoning and the competence-performance issue: a developmental analysis of a training task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 274–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overton, W. F., Ward, S. L., Black, J., Noveck, I. A. & O'Brien, D. P. (1987). Form and content in the development of deductive reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 23 (1), 22–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.), Carmichael's manual of child psychology, vol. I. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in reflecting abstraction. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Quinn, S. & Markovits, H. (1998). Conditional reasoning, causality, and the structure of semantic memory: strength of association as a predictive factor for content effects. Cognition, 68, B93–B101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholnick, E. K. & Wing, C. S. (1991). Speaking deductively: preschoolers' use of If in conversation and in conditional inference. Developmental Psychology, 27 (2), 249–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simoneau, M. & Markovits, H. (2003). Reasoning with premises that are not empirically true: evidence for the role of inhibition and retrieval. Developmental Psychology, 39 (6), 964–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119 (1), 3–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: implications for the rationality debate?Behavioral-and-Brain-Sciences, 23 (5), 645–726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 321–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venet, M. & Markovits, H. (2001). Understanding uncertainty with abstract conditional premises. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47 (1), 74–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wason, P. C. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 273–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wason, P. C. & Shapiro, D. (1971). Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 63–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anastasi, A. (1982). Psychological testing. 5th edition. New York: Macmillan Publishing Co. Inc.Google Scholar
Anderson, J. A. (1957). Significance of growth and development to the practicing physician. In Harris (1957).
Armstrong, L. (2003). Every second counts: from recovery to victory. London: Yellow Jersey Press.Google Scholar
Axtell, G. (2000). Knowledge, belief, and character: readings in virtue epistemology. New York: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers Inc.Google Scholar
Bainton, R. H. (1950). Here I stand: a life of Martin Luther. New York: Signet.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. (2005). www.inspirationalstories.com/0/91.html
Bickhard, M. (2003). Process and emergence: normative function and representation. In Seibt, J. (ed.), Process theories: crossdisciplinary studies in dynamic categories. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. & Pellegrini, A. (2002). The origins of human nature. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Blank, G. (2002). www.drblank.com/slaws.htm
Bornstein, M. & Lamb, M. (1992). Developmental psychology: an advanced textbook. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brandom, R. (1994). Making it explicit: reasoning, representing, and discursive commitment. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brandom, R. (2000). Articulating reasons: an introduction to inferentialism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brown, T. & Weiss, L. (1987). Structures, procedures, heuristics and affectivity. Archives de Psychologie, 55, 59–94. Reprinted in L. Smith (1992), Jean Piaget: critical assessments, vol. IV. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Changeux, J-P. (2000). Co-author. In Changeux, J-P. & Ricoeur, P., What makes us think?Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cole, M., Cole, S. & Lightfoot, C. (2005). The development of children. 5th edition. New York: Worth Publishing.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (2004). What is evolutionary psychology? Explaining the new science of the mind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Cresswell, M. & Hughes, G. (1996). A new introduction to modal logic. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
Damon, W. (1998). Handbook of child psychology. 5th edition. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1999). The extended phenotype: the long reach of the gene. Revised edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1901). The rules of sociological method. In Lukes, S. (1982). London: Macmillan Press Ltd.Google Scholar
Flavell, J., Miller, P. & Miller, S. (1993). Cognitive development. 3rd edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1964). The basic laws of arithmetic. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1982). Developmental psychology: an introduction. Boston: Little, Brown & Co.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Selten, R. (2001). Bounded rationality: the adaptive toolbox. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (2001). Pathways of knowledge: public and private. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goswami, U. (2002). Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Habermas, J. (1992). Between facts and norms: contributions to a discourse theory of law and democracy. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Harris, D. B. (1957). The concept of development. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Hawking, S. (2001). The universe in a nutshell. London: Bantam Press.Google Scholar
Horty, J. F. (2001). Agency and deontic logic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, D. (1965). Treatise of human nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Humphrey, G. (1963). Thinking. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Isaacs, N. (1951). Critical notice. British Journal of Psychology, 42, 185–8.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1780). On universal practical philosophy. In Heath, P. & Schneewind, J. (1997), Lectures on ethics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1785). Groundwork of the metaphysics of morals. In Paton, H. J. (1948), The moral law. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1800). Introduction to logic. In Abbott, T. K. (1972), Kant's Introduction to logic. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
King, M. L. (1968). The role of the behavioural scientist in the Civil Rights movement. American Psychologist, 23, 180–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1992). The naturalists return. The Philosophical Review, 101, 53, 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korsgaard, C. (1996). The sources of normativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (2000). The road beyond structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Laudan, L. (1996). Beyond positivism and relativism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
LeTour (2005). www.letour.fr/indexus.html
Longuenesse, B. (1998). Kant and the capacity to judge. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
MacIntyre, A. (1998). A short history of ethics. 2nd edition. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. (1998). Babies think before they speak. Human Development, 41, 116–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, R. B. (1993). Modalities: philosophical essays. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mason, H. E. (1996). Moral dilemmas and moral theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: an experimental view. London: Tavistock Publications Ltd.Google Scholar
Milgram, S. (1992). Individual in a social world. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Mooney, C. (2003). www.csicop.org/doubtandabout/polling/
Nadel, I. (2002). Double act: a life of Tom Stoppard. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1998). The rising curve. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Nozick, R. (2001). Invariances. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. E. (1999). Culture, dialecticism, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1918). Recherche. Lausanne: La Concorde.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1953). Origins of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1976). The grasp of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Plotkin, H. (1997). Evolution in mind. London: Allen Lane Press.Google Scholar
Popper, K. R. (1968). The logic of scientific discovery. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1979). Objective knowledge. 2nd edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pufendorf, S. (1703). Of the law of nature and nations. Oxford: Lichfield.Google Scholar
Putnam, H. (2002). The collapse of the fact/value dichotomy, and other essays. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Quine, W. (1972). Methods of logic. 3rd edition. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, P. (2000). Co-author. In Changeux, J-P. & Ricoeur, P., What makes us think?Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ross, A. (1968). Directives and norms. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Sainsbury, R. M. (1991). Logical forms. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. (2007). 30 years on – a large anti-‘Flynn effect’? The Piagetian test volume & heaviness norms 1975–2003. Brilish Journal of Educational Psychology.CrossRef
Siegler, R. S. (1991). Children's thinking. 2nd edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1981). Sciences of the artificial. 2nd edition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviourism. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
Slater, A. & Muir, D. (1999). Blackwell reader in developmental psychology. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (1993). Necessary knowledge. Hove, UK: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2002). Reasoning by mathematical induction in children's arithmetic. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2003). From epistemology to psychology in the development of knowledge. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spencer, R. (1957). Evolution and development: a view of anthropology. In Harris (1957).
Spinoza, B. (1963). Short treatise. New York: Russell & Russell.Google Scholar
Summers, L. H. (2005). Remarks at NBER conference on diversifying the science & engineering workforce. President, Harvard University homepage.
Wason, P. (1977). Self-contradictions. In Johnson-Laird, P. & Wason, P. (eds.), Thinking: readings in cognitive science. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism. New York: Norton & Company Inc.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1963). Norm and action. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1968). The varieties of goodness. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1983). Practical reason. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
, Aristotle (1928). περι ψυξήγ. In Oeuvres complètes. Paris: Guillaume Budé.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1902). Development and evolution. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1906, 1908, 1911). Thoughts and things or genetic logic. 3 vols. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bergson, H. (1911). Creative evolution (trans. A. Mitchell). New York: Holt.Google Scholar
Bosanquet, B. (1888). On the morphology of knowledge. 2 vols. London/New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cassirer, E. (1903–20). Das Erkenntnisproblem. 3 vols. Berlin: B. Cassirer.Google Scholar
Cassirer, E. (1953, 1955, 1957). Philosophy of symbolic forms. 3 vols. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza. London: William Heinemann.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. London: Murray.Google Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1897). Le suicide. Paris: Félix Alcan.Google Scholar
Furth, H. (1986). The social function of Piaget's theory: a response to Apostel. New Ideas, 4 (1), 23–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobhouse, L. T. (1888). Theory of knowledge. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Hobhouse, L. T. (1912). Mind in evolution. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1881). Science and culture. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology. New York: Henry Holt, vol. II.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1972). Introduction to logic. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.Google Scholar
Liard, L. (1873). De Democrito philosopho. Paris.Google Scholar
Lovejoy, A. O. (1936). The great chain of being. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. R. & Varela, F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1936). La naissance de l'intelligence chez l'enfant. Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1953). The origins of intelligence in the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1928). Die Methodenfrage der Denkpsychologie. Vienna: unpublished doctoral dissertation, Vienna University.
Russell, E. (1946). The defectiveness of organic activities. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sigwart, C. (1894–5). Logic. 2 vols. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Thompson, D. A. W. (1942). On growth and form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1983). Practical reason. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1880). Logik. Stuttgart: Enke.Google Scholar
Aristotle (1908). De anima (On the soul). In Ross, W. D. (ed.), The works of Aristotle. Oxford: Clarendon Press, §424a, pp. 17–22.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1980). A model of developmental and psychological processes. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 102, 61–116.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1988). Piaget on variation and selection models: structuralism, logical necessity, and interactivism. Human Development, 31, 274–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1989). The nature of psychopathology. In Simek-Downing, Lynn (ed.), International psychotherapy: theories, research, and cross-cultural implications. Westport, CT: Praeger Press, pp. 115–40.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1992a). How does the environment affect the person? In Winegar, L. T. & Valsiner, J. (eds.), Children's development within social contexts: metatheory and theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 63–92.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1992b). Scaffolding and self scaffolding: central aspects of development. In Winegar, L. T. & Valsiner, J. (eds.), Children's development within social contexts: research and methodology. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 33–52.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1992c). Piaget on variation and selection models: structuralism, logical necessity, and interactivism. In Smith, L. (ed.), Jean Piaget: critical assessments, vol. IV. London: Routledge, ch. 83, pp. 388–434. (Reprint of 1988)Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1992d). Commentary on the age 4 transition. Human Development, 35 (3), 182–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (1993). Representational content in humans and machines. Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence, 5, 285–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2000a). Motivation and emotion: an interactive process model. In Ellis, R. D. & Newton, N. (eds.), The caldron of consciousness. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins, pp. 161–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2000b). Emergence. In Andersen, P. B., Emmeche, C., Finnemann, N. O. & Christiansen, P. V. (eds.), Downward causation. Aarhus, Denmark: University of Aarhus Press, pp. 322–48.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2001). Why children don't have to solve the frame problems: cognitive representations are not encodings. Developmental Review, 21, 224–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2003a). An integration of motivation and cognition. In Smith, L., Rogers, C. & Tomlinson, P. (eds.), Development and motivation: joint perspectives. Leicester: British Psychological Society, Monograph Series II, pp. 41–56.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2003b). Process and emergence: normative function and representation. In Seibt, J. (ed.), Process theories: crossdisciplinary studies in dynamic categories. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic, pp. 121–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (2003c). The biological emergence of representation. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 105–31.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. (in preparation). The whole person.
Bickhard, M. H. & Campbell, R. L. (1989). Interactivism and genetic epistemology. Archives de Psychologie, 57 (221), 99–121.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. & Campbell, R. L. (1996). Topologies of learning and development. New Ideas in Psychology, 14 (2), 111–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickhard, M. H. & Richie, D. M. (1983). On the nature of representation: a case study of James Gibson's theory of perception. New York: Praeger Publishers.Google Scholar
Brady, G. (2000). From Peirce to Skolem. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1974). Evolutionary epistemology. In Schilpp, P. A. (ed.), The Philosophy of Karl Popper. LaSalle, IL: Open Court, pp. 413–63.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1990). Levels of organization, downward causation, and the selection-theory approach to evolutionary epistemology. In G. Greenberg, & Tobach, E. (eds.), Theories of the evolution of knowing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 1–17.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. (1992). Truth and historicity. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. L. & Bickhard, M. H. (1986). Knowing levels and developmental stages. Contributions to Human Development. Basel, Switzerland: Karger.Google Scholar
Campbell, R. L. & Bickhard, M. H. (1992). Types of constraints on development: an interactivist approach. Developmental Review, 12 (3), 311–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caston, V. (1997). Epiphenomenalisms, ancient and modern. Philosophical Review, 106 (3), 309–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, W. D. & Bickhard, M. H. (2002). The process dynamics of normative function. Monist, 85 (1), 3–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coffa, J. A. (1991). The semantic tradition from Kant to Carnap. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1987). A situated grandmother?Mind and Language, 2, 64–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1990a). Information and representation. In Hanson, P. P. (ed.), Information, language, and cognition. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press, pp. 175–90.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1990b). Fodor's guide to mental representation. In Fodor, J. A. (ed.), A theory of mental content and other essays. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (2003). Hume variations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gill, M-L. (1989). Aristotle on substance. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hume, D. (1978). A treatise of human nature. Index by L. A. Selby-Bigge; Notes by P. H. Nidditch. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hylton, P. (1990). Russell, idealism, and the emergence of analytic philosophy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: intentional behavior as a complex system. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Olson, K. R. (1987). An essay on facts. Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in reflecting abstraction, ed. and trans. Robert L. Campbell. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Plato (1892). Theaetetus. In The dialogues of Plato, vol. IV, ed. and trans. B. Jowett. Oxford: Clarendon Press, p. 191.
Rouse, J. (2002). How scientific practices matter: reclaiming philosophical naturalism. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Alston, W. (1989). Epistemic justification. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Apostel, L. et al. (1957). Les liaisons analytiques et synthétiques dans les comportements du sujet. In Etudes d'épistémologie génétique, vol. IV. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
Aristotle (1984). Nichomachean ethics. In Barnes, J. (ed.), The complete works of Aristotle, vol. II. Princeton: Princeton University Press, pp. 1, 729–867.Google Scholar
Armstrong, D. (1973). Belief, truth and knowledge. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Axtell, G. (2000). Knowledge, belief, and character. Lanham, MD: Rowman.Google Scholar
Bicchieri, C. (1993). Rationality and coordination. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bonjour, L. (1985). The structure of empirical knowledge. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Chisholm, R. (1977). Theory of knowledge. 2nd edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. (1979). Scientific realism and the plasticity of mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifford, W. (1999). The ethics of belief and other essays. Amherst, NY: Prometheus.Google Scholar
Conee, E. & Feldman, R. (2004). Evidentialism. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Copp, D. (2001). Morality, normativity, and society. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dancy, J. & Sosa, E. (eds.) (1992). A companion to epistemology. Oxford: Blackwell's.Google Scholar
Danielson, P. (1992). Artificial morality. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1984). Meditations on first philosophy. In Cottingham, J. et al. (eds., trans.), The philosophical writings of Descartes, vol. II. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–62. (Original work published 1641.)Google Scholar
Fairweather, A. & Zagzebski, L. (eds.) (2001). Virtue epistemology: essays on epistemic virtue and responsibility. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gauthier, D. (1986). Morals by agreement. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Ginet, C. (1975). Knowledge, perception and memory. Dordrecht: D. Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. I. (l986). Epistemology and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRef
Goldman, A. I. (1992). Liaisons: philosophy meets the cognitive and social sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (2001). Social epistemology. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/epistemology-social
Hamlyn, D. (1971). Epistemology and conceptual development. In Mischel, T. (ed.), Cognitive development and epistemology. New York: Academic Press, pp. 3–24.
Inhelder, B. (1981). Some aspects of Piaget's genetic approach to cognition. In Furth, H. (ed.), Piaget and knowledge: theoretical foundations. 2nd edition. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 22–39.Google Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (1986). Piaget's theory of knowledge: genetic epistemology and scientific reason. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (1999). The conduct of inquiry: an introduction to logic and scientific method. Lanham, MD: University Press of America.Google Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (ed.) (ed.) (2000). Special issue: Piaget's Sociological studies. New Ideas In Psychology, 18, 119–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (2003). Piaget's social epistemology. In Carpendale, J. & Müller, U. (eds.), Social interaction and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 45–67.Google Scholar
Kitchener, R. F. (forthcoming). Developmental epistemology: cognitive development and naturalistic epistemology. (Book manuscript.)
Kitcher, P. (1992). The naturalists return. Philosophical Review, 101, 53–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kripke, S. A. (1982). Wittgenstein on rules and private language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Laudan, L. (1987). Progress or rationality? The prospects for normative naturalism. American Philosophical Quarterly, 24, 19–31.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1975). An essay concerning human understanding (ed. P. H. Niddith, 4th edition). Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Maffie, J. (1990). Recent work on naturalized epistemology. American Philosophical Quarterly, 27, 281–93.Google Scholar
Opp, D. (1983). Die Enstehung der sozialer Normen. Tübingen: J. C. B. Mohr.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1942). Les trois structures fondamentales de la vie psychique: rythme, régulation et groupement. Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie und ihre Anwendungen, 1, 9–21.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1950). Introduction à l'épistémologie génétique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, vol. I.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1955). Les lignes générales de l'épistémologie génétique. In Actes du deuxième Congrès international de l'Union internationale de philosophie des sciences, Zurich, 1954, vol. I. Neuchâtel: de Griffon, pp. 26–45.
Piaget, J. (1957). Programme et méthodes de l'épistémologie génétique. In Etudes d'épistémologie génétique, vol. I. Epistémologie génétique et recherche psychologique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, pp. 13–84.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Défense de l'épistémologie génétique. In Etudes d'épistémologie génétique, vol. XVI. Implication, formalization et logique naturelle. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, pp. 165–91.
Piaget, J. (1967a). Nature et méthodes de l'épistémologie. In Piaget, J. (ed.), Logique et connaissance scientifique. Paris: Gallimard, pp. 3–61.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1967b). Les méthodes de l'épistémologie. In Piaget, J. (ed.), Logique et connaissance scientifique. Paris: Gallimard, pp. 62–126.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971a). Psychology and epistemology. New York: Viking.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971b). Genetic epistemology. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1972a). Insights and illusions of philosophy (trans. W. Mays, 2nd edition). New York: World Publishing Co. (Original work published 1968.)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1972b). The principles of genetic epistemology (trans. W. Mays). London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1970.)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structures: the central problem of intellectual development (trans. T. Brown & K. J. Thampy). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1975.)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1986). Essay on necessity (trans. L. Smith & F. Steel). Human development, 29, 301–14.CrossRef
Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies (ed. L. Smith, trans. L. Smith et al.). London/New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Plantinga, A. (1993a). Warrant and proper function. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plantinga, A. (1993b). Warrant: the current debate. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1951). Two dogmas of empiricism. Philosophical Review, 60, 20–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quine, W. V. O. (1969). Epistemology naturalized. In his Ontological relativity and other essays. New York: Columbia University Press, pp. 69–90.Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schmitt, F. (1994). Socializing epistemology: an introduction through two sample issues. In Schmitt, F. (ed.), Socializing epistemology: the social dimensions of knowledge. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield, pp. 1–27.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1993). Necessary knowledge. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1995). Introduction. In Smith, L. (ed., trans. L. Smith et al.), Jean Piaget: sociological studies. London: Routledge, pp. 1–22.Google Scholar
Sosa, E. (1991). Knowledge in perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sugden, R. (1986). The economics of rights, cooperation and welfare. Oxford: Blackwell's.Google Scholar
Taylor, M. (1984). The possibility of cooperation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ullmann-Margalit, E. (1977). The emergence of norms. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Zagzebski, L. (1996). Virtues of the mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adey, P. & Shayer, M. (2002). Cognitive acceleration comes of age. In Shayer, M. & Adey, P. (eds.), Learning intelligence. Buckingham: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Aristotle (1987). On the soul. In Ackrill, J. (ed.), New Aristotle reader. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Audi, R. (1999). The Cambridge dictionary of philosophy. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bell, V. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1998). A model theory of modal reasoning. Cognitive Science, 22, 25–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bereiter, C. (2002). Education and mind in the knowledge age.Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bickhard, M. (2003). The biological emergence of representation. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bjorklund, D. & Pellegrini, A. (2002). The origins of human nature.Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Braine, M. & O'Brien, D. (1998). Mental logic. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Brandom, R. (1994). Making it explicit: reasoning, representing, and discursive commitment.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Brandom, R. (2000). Articulating reasons: an introduction to inferentialism. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a theory of instruction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bryant, P. (2001). Learning in Geneva: the contribution of Bärbel Inhelder and her colleagues. In Typhon, A. & Vonèche, J. (eds.), Working with Piaget: essays in honour of Bärbel Inhelder. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Byrnes, J. & Duff, M. (1989). Young children's comprehension of modal expressions. Cognitive Development, 4, 369–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carl, W. (1994). Frege's theory of sense and reference: its origins and scope. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, L. (1895). What the tortoise said to Achilles. Mind, 4, 278–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D. (1996). The conscious mind: in search of a fundamental theory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Changeux, J-P. (2000). Co-author. In Changeux, J-P. & Ricoeur, P., What makes us think?Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Cheng, P. & Holyoak, K. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17, 391–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition, 31, 187–276.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (2004). What is evolutionary psychology? Explaining the new science of the mind. New Haven: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Cresswell, M. & Hughes, G. (1996). A new introduction to modal logic. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1999). The extended phenotype: the long reach of the gene. Revised edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1931). Meditations on first philosophy. In Haldane, E. & Ross, G. (eds.), Philosophical works of Descartes, vol. I. New York: Dover Publications.Google Scholar
Dummett, M. (1981). Frege: philosophy of language. 2nd edition. London: Duckworth.Google Scholar
Evans, J. (2002). Logic and human reasoning: an assessment of the deduction paradigm. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 978–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrari, M., Pinard, A. & Runions, K. (2001). Piaget's framework for a scientific study of consciousness. Human Development, 44, 195–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2003). Is there a faculty of deontic reasoning? A critical re-evaluation of abstract deontic versions of the Wason selection task. In Over, D. (ed.). Evolution and the psychology of thinking. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2004). Domains of deontic reasoning: resolving the discrepancy between the cognitive and moral reasoning literatures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 447–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J., Miller, P. & Miller, S. (1993). Cognitive development. 3rd edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1950). The foundations of arithmetic. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1964). The basic laws of arithmetic. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1977). Logical investigations. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Frege, G. (1979). Posthumous papers. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Goldman, A. (2001). Pathways of knowledge: public and private. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gruber, H. & Vonèche, J. (1995). The essential Piaget. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson Inc.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. & Núñez, M. (1996). Understanding of permission rules by preschool children. Child Development, 67, 1572–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawking, S. (2001). The universe in a nutshell. London: Bantam Press.Google Scholar
Holbo, J. (2002). Moral dilemma and the logic of obligation. American Philosophical Quarterly, 39, 259–74.Google Scholar
Horty, J. F. (2001). Agency and deontic logic. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, G. (1951). Thinking. New York: John Wiley.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1868). A liberal education. In Bibby, C. (1971), Huxley on education. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Isaacs, N. (1951). Critical notice. British Journal of Psychology, 42, 185–8.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. (1999). Deductive reasoning. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 109–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kant, I. (1993). Critique of practical reason. 3rd edition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1997). Lectures on ethics. In Collected works. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenny, A. (1995). Frege. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Kitcher, P. (1992). The naturalists return. The Philosophical Review, 101, 53–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klahr, D. (1999). The conceptual habitat: in what kind of system can concepts develop? In Scholnick, E., Nelson, K., Gelman, S. & Miller, P. (eds.), Conceptual development: Piaget's legacy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Korsgaard, C. (1996). The sources of normativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kripke, S. (1982). Wittgenstein on rules and private language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. (1977). The essential tension. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kusch, M. (1995). Psychologism: a case study in the sociology of philosophical knowledge. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lakatos, I. (1974). Falsification and the methodology of scientific research programmes. In Lakatos, I. & Musgrave, A. (eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Macnamara, J. (1994). The logical foundations of cognition. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mason, H. E. (1996). Moral dilemmas and moral theory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Morf, A. (1957). Les relations entre la logique et la langage lors du passage du raisonnement concrèt au raisonnement formel. In Apostel, L., Mandelbrot, B. & Morf, A., Logique, langage, et théorie de l'information. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Morgan, P. (2005). The insider. London: Ebury Press.Google Scholar
Moser, P. (1999). Epistemology. In Audi, R. (ed.), The Cambridge dictionary of philosophy. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1995). Other minds: critical essays 1969–94. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Peng, K. & Nisbett, R. (1999). Culture, dialectics, and reasoning about contradiction. American Psychologist, 54, 741–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1918). Recherche. Lausanne: La Concorde.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1950). Introduction à l'épistémologie génétique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, vol. I.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The child's conception of number. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1953). Logic and psychology. Manchester: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1965). Discussion: genèse et structure en psychologie. In Gandillac, M. & Goldman, L. (eds.), Entretiens sur les notions de genèse et de structure. Paris: Mouton & Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1966). Part II. In Beth, E. & Piaget, J., Mathematical epistemology and psychology. Dordrecht: Reidel.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970a). Genetic epistemology. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970b). A conversation with Jean Piaget. Psychology Today, 3, 25–32.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and knowledge. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1973). Main trends in psychology. London: George Allen & Unwin.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1974). The child and reality. London: Frederick Muller Ltd.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1985). Equilibration of cognitive structures. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1986). Essay on necessity. Human Development, 29, 301–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in reflecting abstraction. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (2006). Reason. New Ideas in Psychology.CrossRef
Piaget, J. & Garcia, J. (1991). Toward a logic of meanings. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1979). Objective knowledge. 2nd edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Priest, G. (1998). What is so bad about contradictions?The Journal of Philosophy, 95, 410–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (2002). The collapse of the fact/value dichotomy, and other essays. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Quine, W. (1972). Methods of logic. 3rd edition. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Ricoeur, P. (2000). Co-author. In Changeux, J-P. & Ricoeur, P., What makes us think?Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Ross, A. (1968). Directives and norms. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Sainsbury, R. M. (1991). Logical forms. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Scholnick, E. & Wing, C. (1995). Logic in conversation: comparative studies of deduction in children and adults. Cognitive Development, 10, 319–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Searle, J. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (1999). Mind, language and society. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Searle, J. (2001). Rationality in action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. (1997). Piaget and Vygotsky: a necessary marriage for effective intervention. In Smith, L., Dockrell, J. & Tomlinson, P. (eds.), Piaget, Vygotsky and beyond. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. & Adey, P. (1981). Towards a science of science teaching. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. & Adey, P. (2002). Learning intelligence. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.Google Scholar
Shayer, M. & Adhami, M. (2003). Realising the cognitive potential of children 5–7 with a mathematics focus. International Journal of Educational Research, 39, 743–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sluga, H. (1980). Gottlob Frege. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1993). Necessary knowledge. Hove, UK: Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (1998). On the development of mental representation. Developmental Review, 18, 202–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (1999a). What Piaget learned from Frege. Developmental Review, 19, 133–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (1999b). Epistemological principles for developmental psychology in Frege and Piaget. New Ideas in Psychology, 17, 83–117, 137–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (1999c). Representation and knowledge are not the same thing. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 22, 784–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (2002a). Reasoning by mathematical induction in children's arithmetic. Oxford: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2002b). Piaget's model. In Goswami, U. (ed.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, L. (2003). From epistemology to psychology in the development of knowledge. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2004). Developmental epistemology and education. In Carpendale, J. & Müller, U. (eds.), Social interaction and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wason, P. (1966). Reasoning. In Foss, B. (ed.), New horizons in psychology. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin.Google Scholar
Wason, P. (1977). The theory of formal operations: a critique. In Gerber, B. (ed.), Piaget and knowing. LondonRoutledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical investigations. 2nd edition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1978). Remarks on the foundations of mathematics. 3rd edition. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1963). Norm and action. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1983a). Practical reason. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1983b). Truth, knowledge, and modality. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1985). The tree of knowledge. Leiden: E. J. Brill.Google Scholar
Boesch, E. E. (1991). Symbolic action theory and cultural psychology. Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breit, H. & Eckensberger, L. H. (2004). Die Faktizität des Normenbewusstseins: eine entwicklungs psychologische Perspektive. In Lütge, C. & Vollmer, G. (eds.), Fakten statt Normen? Zur Rolle einzelwissenschaftlicher Argumente in einer naturalistischen Ethik. Baden-Baden: Nomos-Verlag, pp. 207–24.Google Scholar
Cole, M. (1998). Cultural psychology: a once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dasen, P. R. & Heron, A. (1981). Cross-cultural tests of Piaget's theory. In Triandis, H. C. & Heron, A. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, vol. IV: Developmental psychology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, pp. 295–342.Google Scholar
Day, J. & Tappan, M. (1996). The narrative approach to moral development: from the epistemic subject to dialogical selves. Human Development, 32, 67–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1986). Handlung, Konflikt und Reflexion: zur Dialektik von Struktur und Inhalt im moralischen Urteil. In Edelstein, W. & Nunner-Winkler, G. (eds.), Zur Bestimmung der Moral: philosophische und sozialwissenschaftliche Beiträge zur Moralforschung. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, pp. 409–42.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1990). From cross-cultural psychology to cultural psychology. The Quarterly Newsletter of the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition, 12 (1), 37–52.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1993). Normative und deskriptive, strukturelle und empirische Anteile in moralischen Urteilen: ein Ökonomie-Ökologie Konflikt aus psychologischer Sicht. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Gähde, U. (eds.), Ethische Norm und empirische Hypothese. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp Verlag, pp. 328–79.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1995). Activity or action: two different roads towards an integration of culture into psychology?Culture & Psychology, 1, 67–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (1996). Agency, action and culture: three basic concepts for cross-cultural psychology. In Pandey, J., Sinha, D. & Bhawuk, D. P. S. (eds.), Asian contributions to cross-cultural psychology. New Delhi: Sage Publications, pp. 72–102.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. (2003). Wanted: a contextualized psychology: a plea for cultural psychology based on actual psychology. In Saraswathi, T. S. (ed.), Cross-cultural perspectives in human development: theory, research, and application. New Delhi: Sage Publications, pp. 70–101.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. & Emminghaus, W. B. (1982). Moralisches Urteil und Aggression: zur Systematisierung und Präzisierung des Aggressionskonzeptes sowie einiger empirischer Befunde. In Hilke, R. & Kempf, W. (eds.), Aggression: naturwissenschaftliche und kulturwissenschaftliche Perspektiven der Aggressionsforschung. Bern: Huber, pp. 208–80.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. & Meacham, J. (1984). The essentials of action theory: a framework for discussion. Human Development, 27, 166–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Eckensberger, L. H. & Plath, I. (in press). Soziale Kognitionen. In Schneider, W. & Sodian, B. (eds.), Enzyklopädie für Psychologie, Kognitive Entwicklung, C Theorie und Forschung, Serie V Entwicklung, vol. II. Göttingen: Hogrefe, pp. 407–91.
Eckensberger, L. H. & Reinshagen, H. (1980). Kohlbergs Stufentheorie der Entwicklung des moralischen Urteils: ein Versuch ihrer Reinterpretation im Bezugsrahmen handlungstheoretischer Konzepte. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Silbereisen, R. K. (eds.), Entwicklung sozialer Kognitionen: Modelle, Theorien, Methoden, Anwendung. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta, pp. 65–131.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. & Zimba, R. F. (1997). The development of moral judgment. In Dasen, P. & Saraswathi, T. S. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology. 2nd edition, vol. II. Developmental psychology. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, pp. 299–338.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H., Breit, H. & Döring, T. (1996). Moral judgments in the context of ecological and economical value orientations: the case of a coal-fuelled power station. Paper read at the 22nd Annual Conference of the Association for Moral Education (AME) Ottawa, November, 1996.
Eckensberger, L. H., Breit, H. & Döring, T. (1997). Moral judgments and knowledge in the context of ecological and economical value orientations: the case of a coal-fuelled power plant. Paper read at the 23rd Annual Conference of the Association for Moral Education (AME) Atlanta, November, 1997.
Eckensberger, L. H., Breit, H. & Döring, T. (1999a). Ethik und Barrieren in umweltbezogenen Entscheidungen: eine entwicklungspsychologische Perspektive. In Linneweber, V. & Kals, E. (eds.), Umweltgerechtes Handeln. Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 165–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H., Breit, H.& Döring, T. (1999b). The role of affects in contextualized moral judgments. Paper read at the 25th Annual Conference of the Association for Moral Education (AME) Minneapolis, November, 1999.
Eckensberger, L. H., Döring, T. & Breit, H. (2001). Moral dimensions in risk evaluation. Research in Social Problems and Public Policy, 9, 137–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelhardt, H. D. (1975). Umweltstrategie: Materialien und Analysen zu einer Umweltethik in der Industriegesellschaft. Gütersloh: Mohn.Google Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the life-cycle. New York: International University Press.Google Scholar
Fowler, J. W. (1981). Stages of faith. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1998). Surrogates for theories. Theory & Psychology, 8 (2), 195–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haan, N. (1977). Coping and defending: processes of self-environment organization. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Habermas, J. (1981). Theorie des kommunikativen Handelns, vol. I: Handlungsrationalität und geschlechtliche Rationalisierung. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1966). Groundwork of the metaphysics of morals. In Paton, H. (ed.), The moral law. London: Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Kasper, E., Sieloff, U., Nieder, A. & Eckensberger, L. H. (1987). Entwicklung eines Tests zur Erfassung des Wissens über die Kraftwerksproblematik. Arbeiten der Fachrichtung Psychologie, Universität des Saarlandes, 114.
Kegan, R. (1982). The emerging self. Harvard: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kesselring, T. (1981). Entwicklung und Widerspruch: ein Vergleich zwischen Piagets genetischer Erkenntnistheorie und Hegels Dialektik. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1973). The claim to moral adequacy of a highest stage of moral judgment. Journal of Philosophy, 70, 630–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. & Launier, R. (1978). Stress-related transactions between person and environment. In Pervin, L. A. & Lewis, M. (eds.), Perspective in interactional psychology. New York: Plenum, pp. 287–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leontiev, A. N. (1977). Tätigkeit, Bewusstsein, Persönlichkeit (Activity, consciousness, personality). Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta.Google Scholar
Lourenço, O. (1996). Reflections on narrative approaches to moral development. Human Development, 39, 83–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meggle, G. (1993). Semantische, empirische und normative Aspekte eines Begriffs des kommunikativen Handelns. In Eckensberger, L. H. & Gähde, U. (eds.), Ethische Norm und empirische Hypothese. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, pp. 197–221.
Oser, F. & Gmünder, P. (1984). Der Mensch: Stufen seiner religiösen Entwicklung. Zürich: Benziger.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1918). Recherche. Lausanne: La Concorde.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.), Carmichael's handbook of child psychology, vol. I. New York: Wiley, pp. 703–32.
Piaget, J. (1977). The grasp of consciousness. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul ((1974). La prise de conscience. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France).
Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity: their relationship during child development, trans. and ed. Brown, T. A. & Kaegi, C. E.. Palo Alto, California: Annual Reviews Inc.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1995). Sociological studies. London: Routledge ((1965). Etudes sociologiques. Geneva: Droz).
Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in reflecting abstraction. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Rosnow, R. L. & Georgoudi, M. (1986). The spirit of contextualism. In Rosnow, R. L. & Georgoudi, M. (eds.), Contextualism and understanding in behavioral science. New York: Praeger, pp. 3–22.Google Scholar
Schwartz, L. (1951). Die Neurosen und die dynamische Psychologie von Pierre Janet. Basel, Switzerland: Schwabe.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1990). Cultural psychology – what is it? In Stigler, J. W., Shweder, R. A. & Herdt, G. (eds.), Cultural psychology: essays on comparative human development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 1–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sieloff, U., Schirk, S., Kasper, E., Nieder, A. & Eckensberger, L. H. (1988). Eine textanalytische Auswertungstechnik zur Erfassung von Abwehr und Bewältigung beim moralischen Argumentieren. Arbeiten der Fachrichtung Psychologie, 118, Saarbrücken: Universität des Saarlandes.
Valsiner, J. (1987). Culture and the development of children's action. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1972). Tractatus logico-philosophicus. 2nd edition. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Wright, G. H. (1971). Explanation and understanding. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Arsenio, W. F. & Lover, A. (1995). Children's conceptions of sociomoral affect: happy victimizers, mixed emotions, and other expectancies. In Killen, M. & Hart, D. (eds.), Morality in everyday life. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 87–128.Google Scholar
Barrett, C. D., Keller, M., Takezawa, M. & Wichary, S. (submitted). Children's understanding of social emotions: a study in ecological rationality.
Berndt, T. J. (1993). The morality of friendship versus the morality of individual autonomy. Paper presented at the symposium ‘Spheres of Morality’ at the 60th anniversary meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans, March, 1993.
Bersoff, D. M. & Miller, J. G. (1993). Culture, context and the development of moral accountability judgments. Developmental Psychology, 29, 664–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: a critical review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 1–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blasi, A. (2004). Moral functioning: moral understanding and personality. In Lapsley & Narváez (eds.), pp. 335–48.
Blum, L. A. (1980). Friendship, altruism and morality. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Bond, M. H. (ed.) (1996). The handbook of Chinese psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Boyes, M. & Walker, L. J. (1988). Implications of cultural diversity for the universality claims of Kohlberg's theory of moral reasoning. Human Development, 31, 44–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bukowski, W. M. & Sippola, L. K. (1996). Friendship and morality: how are they related? In Bukowski, W. M., Newcomb, A. F. & Hartup, W. W. (eds.), The company they keep. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 238–61.Google Scholar
Colby, A. & Kohlberg, L. (1987). The measurement of moral judgment, vol. I. Theoretical foundations and research validation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for social exchange. In Barkow, J., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (eds.), The adapted mind: evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 163–228.Google Scholar
Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Döbert, R. (1989). Moralische Erziehung in der Volksrepublik China (Moral education in the People's Republic of China). Vergleichende Pädagogik, 25, 246–59.Google Scholar
Eckensberger, L. H. & Zimba, R. F. (1997). The development of moral judgment. In Dasen, P. R. & Saraswathi, T. S. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, vol. III. Developmental psychology. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, pp. 299–338.Google Scholar
Edelstein, W. & Noam, G. G. (1982). Regulatory structures of the self and ‘postformal’ stages in adulthood. Human Development, 25, 407–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (1982). The development of reasoning regarding prosocial behavior. In Eisenberg, N. (ed.), The development of prosocial behavior. New York: Academic Press, pp. 219–49.Google Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2004). Domains of deontic reasoning: resolving the discrepancy between the cognitive and moral reasoning literatures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 447–74.CrossRef
Frankena, W. K. (1973). Ethics. 2nd edition. London: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Gerson, R. R. & Damon, W. (1978). Moral understanding and children's conduct. In Damon, W. (ed.), Moral development. New directions for child development, No. 2. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass, pp. 41–61.Google Scholar
Gibbard, A. (1992). Wise choices, apt feelings: a theory of normative judgement. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, J. C. & Widaman, K. F. (1982). Social intelligence: measuring the development of sociomoral reflection. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: psychological theory and women's development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, R. & So-Kum Tang, C. (1996). Chinese personal relationships. In Bond (ed.), pp. 294–308.
Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity: a preliminary statement. American Sociological Review, 25, 161–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: implications for caring and justice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M. (1984). Resolving conflicts in friendship: the development of moral understanding in everyday life. In Kurtines, W. M. & Gewirtz, J. L. (eds.), Morality, moral behaviour and moral development. New York: Wiley, pp. 140–58.Google Scholar
Keller, M. (1996). Moralische Sensibilität: Entwicklung in Freundschaft und Familie (Moral sensibility: development in friendship and family). Weinheim: Psychologie-Verlags Union.Google Scholar
Keller, M., in cooperation with Cecora, L., Eckert, U., Fang, Fu-xi & Fang, Ge (2004a). Self in relationship. In Lapsley & Narváez (eds.), pp. 267–98.
Keller, M. & Edelstein, W. (1990). The emergence of morality in personal relationships. In Wren, T. E. (ed.), The moral domain: essays in the ongoing discussion between philosophy and the social sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 255–82.Google Scholar
Keller, M. & Edelstein, W. (1991). The development of socio-moral meaning making: domains, categories and perspective-taking. In Kurtines, W. M. & Gewirtz, J. L. (eds.), Handbook of moral behavior and development, vol. II. Research. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 89–114.Google Scholar
Keller, M. & Edelstein, W. (1993). The development of the moral self from childhood to adolescence. In Noam & Wren (eds.), pp. 310–36.
Keller, M. & Reuss, S. (1984). An action-theoretical reconstruction of the development of social-cognitive competence. Human Development, 27, 211–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M., Eckensberger, L. H. & Rosen, K. (1989). A critical note on the conception of preconventional morality: the case of stage 2 in Kohlberg's theory. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 57–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M., Gummerum, M., Wang, X. T. & Lindsey, S. (2004b). Understanding perspectives and emotions in contract violation: development of deontic and moral reasoning. Child Development, 75, 614–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, M., Lourenço, O., Malti, T. & Saalbach, H. (2003). The multifaceted phenomenon of ‘happy victimizers’: a cross-cultural comparison. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, M., Edelstein, W., Fang, Fu-xi, Hong, Tang & Schuster, P. (1996). The role of culture in the attribution of moral feelings. Paper presented at the 23rd Annual Conference of the Association for Moral Education, Ottawa, November, 1996.
Keller, M., Edelstein, W., Krettenauer, T., Fang, F. & Fang, G. (2005). Reasoning about moral obligations and interpersonal responsibilities in different cultural contexts. In W. Edelstein & G. Nunner-Winkler (eds.), Morality in context. Advances in Psychology 137. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 317–40.CrossRef
Keller, M., Edelstein, W., Schmid, C., Fang, F. & Fang, G. (1998). Reasoning about responsibilities and obligations in close relationships: a comparison across two cultures. Developmental Psychology, 34, 731–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohlberg, L. (1976). Moral stages and moralization: the cognitive developmental approach. In Lickona, T. (ed.), Moral development and behavior: theory, research and social issues. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, pp. 31–53.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1984). Essays on moral development, vol. II. The psychology of moral development: the nature and validity of moral stages. San Francisco: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Krappmann, L. (1996). Amicitia, drujba, shin-yu, philia, Freundschaft, friendship: on the cultural diversity of a human relationship. In Bukowski, W. M., Newcomb, A. F. & Hartup, W. W. (eds.), The company they keep: friendship in childhood and adolescence. New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 19–40.Google Scholar
Krettenauer, T. (2004). Metaethical cognition and epistemic reasoning development in adolescence. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 68, 461–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krettenauer, T. & Eichler, D. (2005). Adolescents' self-attributed emotions following a moral transgression: relations with delinquency, confidence in moral judgment, and age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology.
Lapsley, D. K. & Narváez, D. (eds.) (2004). Moral development, self and identity. Mahwah: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ma, H. K. (1988). The Chinese perspectives on moral judgment development. International Journal of Psychology, 23, 201–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisan, M. (2004). Judgement and choice in moral functioning. In Lapsley & Narváez (eds.), pp. 133–60.
Noam, G. G. & Wren, T. E. (1993) in cooperation with Nunner-Winkler, G. & Edelstein, W. (eds.), The moral self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Nucci, L. P. & Lee, J. (1993). Morality and personal autonomy. In Noam & Wren (eds.), pp. 123–48.
Nunner-Winkler, G. (1993). The growth of moral motivation. In Noam & Wren (eds.), pp. 269–91.
Nunner-Winkler, G. & Sodian, B. (1988). Children's understanding of moral emotions. Child Development, 59, 323–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oser, F. & Reichenbach, R. (2005). Moral resilience – the unhappy moralist. In W. Edelstein & G. Nunner-Winkler (eds.), Morality in context. Advances in Psychology 137. Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 203–24.CrossRef
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Free Press. (Original work published 1932.)Google Scholar
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Richards, D. A. J. (1971). A theory of reasons for action. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understanding: developmental and clinical analyses. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M. & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and moral development. In Kagan, J. and Lamb, S. (eds.), The emergence of morality in young children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, pp. 1–83.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. G. (1982). Concepts of self and morality. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1990). Cross-cultural studies of individualism and collectivism. In Berman, J. J. (ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation 1989, vol. XXXVII. Cross-cultural perspectives. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, pp. 41–133.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). Domains and categories in social cognitive development. In Overton, W. F. (ed.), The relationship between social and cognitive development. Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp. 53–90.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In Damon, W. (series ed.) & Eisenberg, N. (vol. ed.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. III. Social, emotional and personality development. 5th edition. New York: Wiley, pp. 863–932.Google Scholar
Youniss, J. (1980). Parents and peers in social development. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Abu-Lughod, L. (1993). Writing women's worlds: Bedouin stories. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Antipoff, H. (1928). Observations sur la compassion et le sens de la justice chez l'enfant. Archives de Psychologie, 21.Google Scholar
Asch, S. E. (1952). Social psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, R. (1934). Patterns of culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Bok, S. (1978/1999). Lying: moral choice in public and private life. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Clémence, A., Doise, W., Rosa, A. S. & Gonzalez, L. (1995). La représentation sociale des droits de l'homme: une recherche internationale sur l'étendue et les limites de l'universalité. Journal Internationale de Psychologie, 30, 181–212.Google Scholar
Davidson, P., Turiel, E. & Black, A. (1983). The effect of stimulus familiarity on the use of criteria and justifications in children's social reasoning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 49–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doise, W., Clémence, A. & Spini, D. (1996). Human rights and social psychology. The British Psychological Society Social Psychology Section Newsletter, 35, 3–21.Google Scholar
Dworkin, R. (1977). Taking rights seriously. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2004). Domains of deontic reasoning: resolving the discrepancy between the cognitive and moral reasoning literatures. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57A, 447–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiddick, L. (2005). Domains of deontic reasoning: a postscript. Paper presented at the 35th Annual Meeting of the Jean Piaget Society. Vancouver, BC, Canada, June 2005.
Freeman, V. G., Rathore, S. S., Weinfurt, K. P., Schulman, K. A. & Sulmasy, D. P. (1999). Lying for patients: physician deception of third-party payers. Archives of Internal Medicine, 159, 2,263–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gewirth, A. (1982). Human rights: essays on justification and applications. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108, 814–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatch, E. (1983). Culture and morality: the relativity of values in anthropology. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Helwig, C. C. (1995). Adolescents' and young adults' conceptions of civil liberties: freedom of speech and religion. Child Development, 66, 152–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helwig, C. C. (1997). The role of agent and social context in judgments of freedom of speech and religion. Child Development, 68, 484–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyman, H. H. & Sheatsley, P. B. (1953). Trends in public opinion on civil liberties. Journal of Social Issues, 9, 6–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
King, M. L. Jr. (1968). The role of the behavioral scientist in the civil rights movement. American Psychologist, 23, 180–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: the cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In Goslin, D. (ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally, pp. 347–480.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1971). From is to ought: how to commit the naturalistic fallacy and get away with it in the study of moral development. In Mischel, T. (ed.), Psychology and genetic epistemology. New York: Academic Press, pp. 151–235.Google Scholar
McClosky, M. & Brill, A. (1983). Dimensions of tolerance: what Americans believe about civil liberties. New York: Russell Sage.Google Scholar
Nucci, L. P. (2001). Education in the moral domain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L. P. & Nucci, M. S. (1982a). Children's responses to moral and social conventional transgressions in free-play settings. Child Development, 53, 1,337–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nucci, L. P. & Nucci, M. S. (1982b). Children's social interactions in the context of moral and conventional transgressions. Child Development, 53, 403–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perkins, S. A. (2003). Adolescent reasoning about lying in close relationships. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of California, Berkeley.
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1951/1995). Egocentric thought and sociocentric thought. In Piaget, J., Sociological studies. London: Routledge, pp. 270–86.Google Scholar
Pizarro, D. A. & Bloom, P. (2003). The intelligence of the moral intuitions: comment on Haidt (2001). Psychological Review, 110, 193–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Protho, J. W. & Grigg, C. M. (1960). Fundamental principles of democracy: bases of agreement and disagreement. Journal of Politics, 22, 276–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarat, A. (1975). Reasoning in politics: the social, political, and psychological bases of principled thought. American Journal of Political Science, 19, 247–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smetana, J. G. (1995). Morality in context: abstractions, ambiguities, and applications. In Vasta, R. (ed.), Annals of child development, vol. X. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers, pp. 83–130.Google Scholar
Smetana, J. G. (2002). Culture, autonomy, and personal jurisdiction in adolescent–parent relationships. In Reese, H. W. & Kail, R. (eds.), Advances in child development and behavior, vol. XXIX. New York: Academic Press, pp. 51–87.Google Scholar
Smith, L. (2003). From epistemology to psychology in the development of knowledge. In Brown, T. & Smith, L. (eds.), Reductionism and the development of knowledge. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 201–28.Google Scholar
Stouffer, S. (1955). Communism, conformity and civil liberties. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Tisak, M. S. & Turiel, E. (1984). Children's conceptions of moral and prudential rules. Child Development, 55, 1,030–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: morality and convention. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1998). The development of morality. In W. Damon (ed.), Handbook of child psychology. 5th edition, vol. III: N. Eisenberg (ed.), Social, emotional, and personality development. New York: Wiley, pp. 863–932.
Turiel, E. (2002). The culture of morality: social development, context, and conflict. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. (2003). Resistance and subversion in everyday life. Journal of Moral Education, 32, 115–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. (2005). Thought about actions in social domains: morality, social conventions, and social interactions. Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley.
Turiel, E. (2006). Thought, emotions, and social interactional processes in moral development. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. G. (eds.), Handbook of moral development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 7–35.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. & Perkins, S. A. (2004). Flexibilities of mind: conflict and culture. Human Development, 47, 158–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E. & Wainryb, C. (1998). Concepts of freedoms and rights in a traditional hierarchically organized society. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 375–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turiel, E., Perkins, S. A. & Mensing, J. F. (in preparation). Judgments about deception in marital relationships. University of California, Berkeley.
Wason, P. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 273–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wertheimer, M. (1935). Some problems in the theory of ethics. Social Research, 2, 353–67.Google Scholar
Wikan, U. (1996). Tomorrow, God willing: self-made destinies in Cairo. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wynia, M. K., Cummins, D. S., VanGeest, J. B. & Wilson, I. B. (2000). Physician manipulation of reimbursement rules for patients: between a rock and a hard place. Journal of the American Medical Association, 283, 1,858–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Borsboom, D. (2005). Measuring the mind: conceptual issues in contemporary psychometrics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erneling, C. E. & Johnson, D. M. (eds.) (2005). The mind as a scientific object: between brain and culture. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Habermas, J. (1987). Theorie des kommunikativen Handelns, vol. I. Handlungsrationalität und gesellschaftliche Rationalisierung, vol. II. Zur Kritik der funktionalistischen Vernunft. 4th edition. Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp.
Heidelberger, M. (2004). Nature from within: Gustav T. Fechner and his psychophysical worldview. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Kusch, M. (1995). Psychologism: a case study in the sociology of philosophical knowledge. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Lord, F. M. & Novick, M. R. (1968). Statistical theories of mental test scores. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
McCarthy, G. E. (2001). Objectivity and the silence of reason: Weber, Habermas, and the methodological disputes in German sociology. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Molenaar, P. C. M. (2003). State space techniques in structural equation modeling: transformation of latent variables in and out of latent variable models. http://www.hhdev.psu.edu/hdfs/faculty/molenaar.html
Molenaar, P. C. M. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2, 201–18.Google Scholar
Nesselroade, J. R. & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2003). Quantitative models for developmental processes. In Valsiner, J. & Connolly, K. (eds.), Handbook of developmental psychology. London: Sage, pp. 622–39.Google Scholar
Oddie, G. (2005). Value, reality, and desire. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (2004). The collapse of the fact/value dichotomy and other essays. 3rd edition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Acredolo, C., O'Connor, J., Banks, L. & Horobin, K. (1989). Children's ability to make probability estimates: skills revealed through application of Anderson's functional measurement methodology. Child Development, 60, 933–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozzi, P. (1990). Fisica ingenua (Naive physics). Milan: Garzanti.Google Scholar
Brainerd, C. J. (1981). Working memory and the developmental analysis of probability judgment. Psychological Review, 88, 463–502.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (1996). Are humans good intuitive statisticians after all? Rethinking some conclusions from the literature on judgment under uncertainty. Cognition, 58, 1–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
da Buti, F. (1852–62). Commento di Francesco da Buti sopra La Divina Commedia di Dante Allighieri (Francesco da Buti's commentary on Dante's Divine Comedy). Pisa: Nistri. (Originally written circa 1385.) Available at http://dciswww.dartmouth.eduGoogle Scholar
David, F. N. (1962). Games, gods and gambling: a history of probability and statistical ideas. London: Griffin.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: how the mind creates mathematics. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
della Lana, J. (1866–7). Comedia di Dante degli Allaghieri col commento di Jacopo della Lana bolognese (Dante's Comedy, commentary by Jacopo della Lana from Bologna). Bologna: Tipografia Regia. (Originally written circa 1324.) Available at http://dciswww.dartmouth.eduGoogle Scholar
Fischbein, E. (1975). The intuitive sources of probabilistic thinking in children. Dordrecht: Reidel.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, J. (2001). The science of conjecture: evidence and probability before Pascal. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. (1993). The bounded rationality of probabilistic mental models. In Manktelow, K. & Over, D. (eds.), Rationality. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Hoffrage, U. (1995). How to improve bayesian reasoning without instruction: frequency format. Psychological Review, 102, 684–704.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilovich, T., Griffin, D. & Kahneman, D. (eds.) (2002). Heuristics and biases: the psychology of intuitive judgment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Girotto, V. (2004). Task understanding. In Leighton, J. & Sternberg, R. (eds.), The nature of reasoning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (2001). Solving probabilistic and statistical problems: a matter of information structure and question form. Cognition, 78, 247–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (2002). Chances and frequencies in probabilistic reasoning: rejoinder to Hoffrage, Gigerenzer, Krauss, and Martignon. Cognition, 84, 353–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (2003). Early reasoning about chances and possibilities: historical and developmental evidence. Paper presented at the 16th Jean Piaget Archives Conference, University of Geneva, September.
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (2005). Probabilistic reasoning and combinatorial analysis. In Girotto, V. & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (eds.), The shape of reason. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Girotto, V. & Gonzalez, M. (in press). Extensional reasoning about chances. In Schaeken, W., Vooght, G., Vandierendonck, A. & d'Ydewalle, G. (eds.), Mental model theory: extensions and refinements. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Hacking, I. (1975). The emergence of probability. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hilton, D. (1995). The social context of reasoning: conversational inference and rational judgement. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 248–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howson, C. & Urbach, P. (1993). Scientific reasoning: the Bayesian approach. Chicago: Open Court.Google Scholar
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1964). The early growth of logic. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1955.)Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N., Legrenzi, P., Girotto, V., Sonino-Legrenzi, M. & Caverni, J. P. (1999). Naive probability: a model theory of extensional reasoning. Psychological Review, 106, 62–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (eds.) (2000). Choices, values, and frames. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kendall, M. G. (1956). The beginnings of a probability calculus. Biometrika, 43, 1–14.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1975). An essay concerning human understanding. Oxford: Clarendon Press. (Original work published 1690.)Google Scholar
Neyman, J. & Pearson, E. S. (1967). Joint statistical papers of J. Neyman and E. S. Pearson. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Krantz, D. H., Jepson, C. & Kunda, D. (1983). The use of statistical heuristics in everyday inductive reasoning. Psychological Review, 90, 339–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ore, O. (1965). Cardano, the gambling scholar. New York: Dover.Google Scholar
Ottimo, A. L. (1827–9). L'Ottimo commento della Divina Commedia (Ottimo's commentary on the Divine Comedy). Pisa: Capurro. (Originally written circa 1331.) Available at http://dciswww.dartmouth.eduGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1975). The origin of the idea of chance in children. New York: Norton. (Original work published 1951.)Google Scholar
Piaget, J. & Szeminska, B. (1952). The child's conception of number. New York: Humanities Press. (Original work published 1941.)Google Scholar
Politzer, G. & Noveck, I. A. (1991). Are conjunction rule violations the results of conversational rule violations?Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 20, 83–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, L. J. (1954). The foundations of statistics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sloman, S. A., Over, D., Slovak, L. & Stibel, J. M. (2003). Frequency illusions and other fallacies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 91, 296–309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tentori, K., Bonini, N. & Osherson, D. (2004). The conjunction fallacy: a misunderstanding about conjunction?Cognitive Science, 26, 467–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A. & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: the conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90, 293–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yost, P. A., Siegel, A. E. & Andrews, J. M. (1962). Nonverbal probability judgments by young children. Child Development, 33, 769–80.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhu, L. & Gigerenzer, G. (2006). Children can solve Bayesian problems: the role of representation in mental computation. Cognition, 98, 287–308.CrossRef
Barrouillet, P. & Lecas, J. F. (1999). Mental models in conditional reasoning and working memory. Thinking and Reasoning, 5 (4), 289–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrouillet, P., Markovits, H. & Quinn, S. (2002). Developmental and content effects in reasoning with causal conditionals. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 235–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D. F. & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1990). The resources construct in cognitive development: diverse sources of evidence and a theory of inefficient inhibition. Developmental Review, 10, 48–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bjorklund, D. F. & Harnishfeger, K. K. (1995). The evolution of inhibition mechanisms and their role in human cognition and behavior. In Dempster, F. N. & Brainerd, C. J. (eds.), Interference and inhibition in cognition. London: Academic Press, pp. 141–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, M. D. S. (1990). The ‘natural logic’ approach to reasoning. In Overton, W. F. (ed.), Reasoning, necessity and logic: developmental perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc., pp. 133–57.Google Scholar
Bucci, W. (1978). The interpretation of universal affirmative propositions. Cognition, 6, 55–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cahan, S. & Artman, L. (1997). Is everyday experience dysfunctional for the development of conditional reasoning?Cognitive Development, 12 (2), 261–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, D. D. (1995). Naive theories and causal deduction. Memory and Cognition, 23 (5), 646–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummins, D. D., Lubart, T., Alksnis, O. & Rist, R. (1991). Conditional reasoning and causation. Memory and Cognition, 19 (3), 274–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawkins, R. (1999). The extended phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
De, Neys W., Schaeken, W. & d'Ydewalle, G. (2002). Causal conditional reasoning and semantic memory retrieval: a test of the ‘semantic memory framework’. Memory and Cognition, 30 (6), 908–20.Google Scholar
Dias, M. G. & Harris, P. L. (1988). The effect of make-believe play on deductive reasoning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 6, 207–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dias, M. G. & Harris, P. L. (1990). The influence of the imagination on reasoning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 305–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, J.St., B. T. & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Evans, J.St., B. T. & Over, D. E. (2004). If. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gigerenzer, G. & Goldstein, D. G. (1996). Reasoning the fast and frugal way: models of bounded rationality. Psychological Review, 103 (4), 650–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hawkins, J., Pea, R. D., Glick, J. & Scribner, S. (1984). ‘Merds that laugh don't like mushrooms’: evidence for deductive reasoning by preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 20 (4), 584–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janveau-Brennan, G. & Markovits, H. (1999). The development of reasoning with causal conditionals. Developmental Psychology, 35 (4), 904–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Byrne, R. M. J. (1991). Deduction. Hillsdale, NJ: Laurence Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. & Byrne, R. M. J. (2002). Conditionals: a theory of meaning, pragmatics, and inference. Psychological Review, 109 (4), 646–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (1996). On the reality of cognitive illusions. Psychological Review, 103 (3), 582–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kail, R. (1992). Processing speed, speech rate, and memory. Developmental Psychology, 28, 899–904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klaczynski, P. A. & Narashimham, G. (1998). Representations as mediators of adolescent deductive reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 5, 865–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kokis, J. V., Macpherson, R., Toplak, M. E., West, R. F. & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Heuristic and analytic processing: age trends and associations with cognitive ability and cognitive styles. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83 (1), 26–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lea, R. B., O'Brien, D. P., Fisch, S., Braine, M. D. S. & Noveck, I. (1990). Predicting propositional logic inferences in text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 361–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leevers, H. & Harris, P. (1999). Transient and persisting effects of instruction on young children's syllogistic reasoning with incongruent and abstract premises. Thinking and Reasoning, 5 (2), 145–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. (1985). Incorrect conditional reasoning among adults: competence or performance?British Journal of Psychology, 76, 241–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. (1995). Conditional reasoning with false premises: fantasy and information retrieval. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 1–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. (2000). A mental model analysis of young children's conditional reasoning with meaningful premises. Thinking and Reasoning, 6 (4), 335–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. & Barrouillet, P. (2002). The development of conditional reasoning: a mental model account. Developmental Review, 22 (1), 5–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. & Doyon, C. (2004). Information processing and reasoning with premises that are not empirically true: interference, working memory and processing speed. Memory and Cognition, 32 (4), 592–601.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. & Handley, S. (in press). Is inferential reasoning just probabilistic reasoning in disguise? Memory and Cognition.
Markovits, H. & Quinn, S. (2002). Efficiency of retrieval correlates with ‘logical’ reasoning from causal conditional premises. Memory and Cognition, 30 (5), 696–706.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H. & Vachon, R. (1989). Reasoning with contrary-to-fact propositions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 398–412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markovits, H. & Vachon, R. (1990). Conditional reasoning, representation and level of abstraction. Developmental Psychology, 26, 942–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markovits, H., Venet, M., Janveau-Brennan, G., Malfait, N., Pion, N. & Vadeboncoeur, I. (1996). Reasoning in young children: fantasy and information retrieval. Child Development, 67, 2,857–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neil, W. T. (1997). Episodic retrieval in negative priming and repetition priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 23 (6), 1,291–3,105.Google Scholar
Oaksford, M., Chater, N. & Larkin, J. (2000). Probabilities and polarity biases in conditional inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 26 (4), 883–99.Google ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, D. P. & Overton, W. F. (1980). Conditional reasoning following contradictory evidence: a developmental analysis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 44–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, D. P. & Overton, W. F. (1982). Conditional reasoning and the competence-performance issue: a developmental analysis of a training task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 34, 274–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Overton, W. F., Ward, S. L., Black, J., Noveck, I. A. & O'Brien, D. P. (1987). Form and content in the development of deductive reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 23 (1), 22–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In Mussen, P. H. (ed.), Carmichael's manual of child psychology, vol. I. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in reflecting abstraction. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Quinn, S. & Markovits, H. (1998). Conditional reasoning, causality, and the structure of semantic memory: strength of association as a predictive factor for content effects. Cognition, 68, B93–B101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scholnick, E. K. & Wing, C. S. (1991). Speaking deductively: preschoolers' use of If in conversation and in conditional inference. Developmental Psychology, 27 (2), 249–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simoneau, M. & Markovits, H. (2003). Reasoning with premises that are not empirically true: evidence for the role of inhibition and retrieval. Developmental Psychology, 39 (6), 964–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119 (1), 3–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanovich, K. E. & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: implications for the rationality debate?Behavioral-and-Brain-Sciences, 23 (5), 645–726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tipper, S. P. (2001). Does negative priming reflect inhibitory mechanisms? A review and integration of conflicting views. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 321–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Venet, M. & Markovits, H. (2001). Understanding uncertainty with abstract conditional premises. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 47 (1), 74–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wason, P. C. (1968). Reasoning about a rule. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 273–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wason, P. C. & Shapiro, D. (1971). Natural and contrived experience in a reasoning problem. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 63–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Edited by Leslie Smith, Lancaster University, Jacques Vonèche, Université de Genève
  • Book: Norms in Human Development
  • Online publication: 22 September 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489778.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Edited by Leslie Smith, Lancaster University, Jacques Vonèche, Université de Genève
  • Book: Norms in Human Development
  • Online publication: 22 September 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489778.012
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Edited by Leslie Smith, Lancaster University, Jacques Vonèche, Université de Genève
  • Book: Norms in Human Development
  • Online publication: 22 September 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489778.012
Available formats
×