Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T12:01:58.618Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

II.I - Clinical Conditions

from Part II - Neurology and Religion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2019

Alasdair Coles
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Joanna Collicutt
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Aftanas, L. I., and Golocheikine, S. A.. 2002. Non-linear dynamic complexity of the human EEG during meditation. Neuroscience Letters 330 : 143–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alajouanine, T. 1963. Dostoiewski’s epilepsy. Brain, 86: 210–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amancio, E. J., Zymberg, S. T and Pires, M. F.. 1994. [Temporal lobe epilepsy and aura with happiness and pleasure: report of 2 cases and review of the literature]. Arquivos de Neuro-Psiquiatria 52: 252–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Armstrong, K. 1983. Beginning the World. New York: St Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
Asheim Hansen, B., and Brodtkorb, E.. 2003. Partial epilepsy with ‘ecstatic’ seizures. Epilepsy and Behavior 4: 667–73.Google Scholar
Aull-Watschinger, S., Pataraia, E. and Baumgartner, C.. 2008. Sexual auras: predominance of epileptic activity within the mesial temporal lobe. Epilepsy and Behavior 12: 124–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bear, D. M., and Fedio, P.. 1977. Quantitative analysis of interictal behavior in temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives of Neurology 34: 454–67.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauregard, M., and Paquette, V.. 2008. EEG activity in Carmelite nuns during a mystical experience. Neuroscience Letters 444: 14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benson, H., et al. 1990. Three case reports of the metabolic and electroencephalographic changes during advanced Buddhist meditation techniques. Journal of Behavioral Medicine 16: 90–5.Google Scholar
Carrazana, E., et al. 1999. Epilepsy and religious experiences: Voodoo possession. Epilepsia 40: 239–41.Google Scholar
Cavanna, A. E., Cavanna, S. and Cavanna, A.. 2010. Epileptic seizures and spirit possession in Haitian culture: report of four cases and review of the literature. Epilepsy and Behavior 19: 8991.Google Scholar
Devinsky, O., and Lai, G. 2008. Spirituality and religion in epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior 12: 636–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewhurst, K., and Beard, A. W.. 1970. Sudden religious conversions in temporal lobe epilepsy. British Journal of Psychiatry 117: 497507.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dolgoff-Kaspar, R., et al. 2011. Numinous-like auras and spirituality in persons with partial seizures. Epilepsia 52: 640–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dostoyevsky, F. 1869. The Idiot. London: Vizetelly.Google Scholar
Fingelkurts, A. A. 2009. Is our brain hardwired to produce God, or is our brain hardwired to perceive God? A systematic review on the role of the brain in mediating religious experience. Cognitive Processing 10: 293326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frank, J. 2010. Dostoevsky: A Writer in His Time. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Geschwind, N. 1979. Behavioural changes in temporal lobe epilepsy. Psychological Medicine 9: 217–19.Google Scholar
Gowers, W. R. 1881. Epilepsy and Other Chronic Convulsive Diseases. London: Churchill.Google Scholar
Greyson, B. B., Derr, L. L. and Fountain, N.. 2015. Mystical experiences associated with seizures. Religion, Brain and Behaviour 5: 182–96.Google Scholar
Griffiths, R.R., et al. 2006. Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology 187: 268–83; discussion 284–92.Google Scholar
Griffiths, R. R., et al. 2011. Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences: immediate and persisting dose-related effects. Psychopharmacology 218: 649–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayton, S., Boylan, L. S., Jackson, S. C., and Devinsky, O.. 2002. Religion and epilepsy. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Academy of Neurology, Denver, CO.Google Scholar
Howden, J. C. 1873. The religious sentiment in epileptics. Journal of Mental Science 18: 482–97.Google Scholar
Hughes, J. R. 2005. The idiosyncratic aspects of the epilepsy of Fyodor Dostoevsky. Epilepsy and Behavior 7: 531–8.Google Scholar
James, W. 1924. The Varieties of Religious Experience. New York: Longmans, Green.Google Scholar
Jilek-Aall, L. 1999. Morbus sacer in Africa: some religious aspects of epilepsy in traditional cultures. Epilepsia 40: 382–6.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. W., Sewell, R. A. and Griffiths, R. R.. 2012. Psilocybin dose-dependently causes delayed, transient headaches in healthy volunteers. Drug and Alcohol Dependency 123: 132–40.Google Scholar
Khwaja, G. A. S., and Chadhury, N.. 2007. Epilepsy and religion. Annals of the Indian Academy of Neurology 10: 165–8.Google Scholar
Le, C., and Silverman, D. H.. 2011. Neuroimaging and EEG-based explorations of cerebral substrates for suprapentasensory perception: a critical appraisal of recent experimental literature. Psychiatry Research 194: 105–10.Google Scholar
Lehmann, D., et al. 2001. Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volitionally meditation-induced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the self. Psychiatry Research 108: 111–21.Google Scholar
Leuba, J. H. 1925. The Psychology of Religious Mysticism. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner.Google Scholar
MacLean, K. A., Johnson, M. W. and Griffiths, R. R.. 2011. Mystical experiences occasioned by the hallucinogen psilocybin lead to increases in the personality domain of openness. Journal of Psychopharmacology 25: 1453–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mungas, D. 1982. Interictal behavior abnormality in temporal lobe epilepsy: a specific syndrome or nonspecific psychopathology? Archives of General Psychiatry 39: 108–11.Google Scholar
O’Connell, K., Keaveney, J. and Paul, R.. 2013. A novel study of comorbidity between schizoaffective disorder and Geschwind syndrome. Case Reports in Psychiatry 2013: 486064.Google Scholar
Ogata, A., and Miyakawa, T.. 1998. Religious experiences in epileptic patients with a focus on ictus-related episodes. Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 52: 321–5.Google Scholar
Oohashi, T., et al. 2002. Electroencephalographic measurement of possession trance in the field. Clinical Neurophysiology 113: 435–45.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. 1955. The twenty-ninth Maudsley lecture: the role of the temporal cortex in certain psychical phenomena. Journal of Mental Science 101: 451–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penfield, W. 1958. Some mechanisms of consciousness discovered during electrical stimulation of the brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 44: 5166.Google Scholar
Persinger, M. A. 1984. People who report religious experiences may also display enhanced temporal-lobe signs. Perceptual and Motor Skills 58: 963–75.Google Scholar
Persinger, M. A. 1991. Preadolescent religious experience enhances temporal lobe signs in normal young adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills 72: 453–4.Google Scholar
Picard, F. 2013. State of belief, subjective certainty and bliss as a product of cortical dysfunction. Cortex 49: 2494–500.Google Scholar
Picard, F., and Craig, A. D.. 2009. Ecstatic epileptic seizures: a potential window on the neural basis for human self-awareness. Epilepsy and Behavior 16: 539–46.Google Scholar
Picard, F., and Kurth, F.. 2014. Ictal alterations of consciousness during ecstatic seizures. Epilepsy and Behavior 30: 5861.Google Scholar
Picard, F., Scavarda, D. and Bartolomei, F.. 2013. Induction of a sense of bliss by electrical stimulation of the anterior insula. Cortex 49: 2935–7.Google Scholar
Postiglione, A., Milan, G, Pappatà, S, De Falco, C., Lamenza, F, Schiattarella, V, Gallotta, G, Sorrentino, P and Striano, S. 2008. Fronto-temporal dementia presenting as Geschwind’s syndrome. Neurocase 14: 264–70.Google Scholar
Reeves, R. R., Kose, S. and Abubakr, A.. 2013. Temporal lobe discharges and glossolalia. Neurocase 20: 236–40.Google Scholar
Reynolds, E. H., and Kinnier Wilson, J. V.. 2008. Psychoses of epilepsy in Babylon: the oldest account of the disorder. Epilepsia 49: 1488–90.Google Scholar
Roberts, J. K., Robertson, M. M. and Trimble, M. R.. 1982. The lateralising significance of hypergraphia in temporal lobe epilepsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 45: 131–8.Google Scholar
Rodin, E., and Schmaltz, S.. 1984. The Bear–Fedio personality inventory and temporal lobe epilepsy. Neurology 34: 591–6.Google Scholar
Surwillo, W. W., and Hobson, D. P.. 1978. Brain electrical activity during prayer. Psychological Reports 43: 135–43.Google Scholar
Tedrus, G. M., Fonseca, L. C. and Hoehr, G. C.. 2014. Spirituality aspects in patients with epilepsy. Seizure 23: 25–8.Google Scholar
Tenke, C. E., Kayser, J., Svob, C., Miller, L., Alvarenga, J. E., Abraham, K., Warner, V., Wickramaratne, P., Weissman, M. M., and Bruder, G. E.. 2017. Association of posterior EEG alpha with prioritization of religion or spirituality: A replication and extension at 20-year follow-up. Biological Psychology 124: 7986.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todman, D. 2008. Epilepsy in the Graeco-Roman world: Hippocratic medicine and Asklepian temple medicine compared. Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 17: 435–41.Google Scholar
Tremont, G., Smith, M. M., Bauer, L., Alosco, M. L., Davis, J. D., Blum, A. S., and LaFrance, W. C. Jr. 2012. Comparison of personality characteristics on the bear-fedio inventory between patients with epilepsy and those with non-epileptic seizures. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 24: 4752.Google Scholar
Trimble, M., and Freeman, A.. 2006. An investigation of religiosity and the Gastaut–Geschwind syndrome in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior 9: 407–14.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M., Novelly, R. A. and Walker, P. J.. 1987. Hyperreligiosity in temporal lobe epilepsy: redefining the relationship. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders 175: 181–4.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. G., and Geschwind, N.. 1975. The interictal behavior syndrome of temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives of General Psychiatry 32: 1580–6.Google Scholar
Williams, D. 1956. The structure of emotions reflected in epileptic experiences. Brain 79: 2967.Google Scholar

References

Foltynie, T., Brayne, C. E. G, Robbins, T. W et al. 2004. The cognitive ability of an incident cohort of Parkinson’s patients in the UK: The CamPaIGN study. Brain 127: 550–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, A. E., and Obeso, J. A.. 2004. Challenges in Parkinson’s disease: restoration of the nigrostriatal dopamine system is not enough. The Lancet Neurology 3: 309–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olanow, C. W., and Obeso, J. A. 2012. The significance of defining preclinical or prodromal Parkinson’s disease. Movement Disorders 27: 666–9.Google Scholar
Del Tredici, K., Rub, U., De Vos, R. A. et al. 2002. Where does Parkinson disease pathology begin in the brain? Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology 61: 413–26.Google Scholar
Spillantini, M. G., Schmidt, M. L., Lee, V. M et al. 1997. Alpha-synuclein in Lewy bodies. Nature 388: 839–40.Google Scholar
IPDGC. 2011. A two-stage meta-analysis identifies several new loci for Parkinson’s disease. PLoS Genetics 7.Google Scholar
Trinh, J., and Farrer, M. 2013. Advances in the genetics of Parkinson disease. Nature Reviews Neurology 9: 445–54.Google Scholar
Kordower, J. H., Olanow, C. W, Dodiya, H. B. et al. 2013. Disease duration and the integrity of the nigrostriatal system in Parkinson’s disease. Brain 136: 2419–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dalley, J., Everitt, B. and Robbins, T.. 2011. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron 69: 680–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jankovic, J., and Poewe, W.. 2012. Therapies in Parkinson’s disease. Current Opinion in Neurology 25: 433–47.Google Scholar
Williams-Gray, C. H., Evans, J. R., Goris, A. et al. 2009. The distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson’s disease: 5 year follow-up of the CamPaIGN cohort. Brain 132: 2958–69.Google Scholar
Weintraub, D., and Burn, D. J. 2011. Parkinson’s disease: the quintessential neuropsychiatric disorder. Movement Disorders 26: 1022–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redgrave, P., Rodriguez, M, Smith, Y et al. 2010. Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: implications for Parkinson’s disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 11: 760–72.Google Scholar
Williams-Gray, C. H., Mason, S. L., Evans, J. R. et al. 2013. The CamPaIGN study of Parkinson’s disease: 10-year outlook in an incident population-based cohort. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 84: 1258–64.Google Scholar
Nombela, C., Rittman, T. and Robbins, T. W. et al. 2011. Multiple modes of impulsivity in Parkinson’s disease. PLoS ONE 9, no. 1: 0085747. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0085747.Google Scholar
Poletti, M., and Bonuccelli, U.. 2012. Personality traits in patients with Parkinson’s disease: assessment and clinical implications. Journal of Neurology 259: 1029–38.Google Scholar
Ishihara, L., and Brayne, C.. 2006. What is the evidence for a premorbid parkinsonian personality: a systematic review. Movement Disorders 21: 1066–72.Google Scholar
Bramley, N., and Eatough, V.. 2005. The experience of living with Parkinson’s disease: an interpretative phenomenological analysis case study. Psychology and Health 20: 223–35.Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, L., Simpson, J. and Smith, A. 2010. A qualitative analysis of mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) in Parkinson’s disease. Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Practice 83: 179–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caap-Ahlgren, M., Lannerheim, L and Dehlin, O. 2002. Older Swedish women’s experiences of living with symptoms related to Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Advanced Nursing 39: 8795.Google Scholar
Stanley-Hermanns, M., and Engebretson, J. 2010. Sailing the stormy seas: the illness experience of persons with Parkinson’s disease. The Qualitative Report 15: 340–69.Google Scholar
Paterson, K. 2012. It’s about Time! Understanding the Experience of Speech Impairment. Abingdon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Park, C. L. 2013. The meaning making model: a framework for understanding meaning, spirituality, and stress-related growth in health psychology. The European Health Psychologist 15: 40–7.Google Scholar
Collicutt McGrath, J. 2011. Posttraumatic growth and spirituality after brain injury. Brain Impairment 12: 8292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Calhoun, L. G., Cann, A, Tedeschi, R. G et al. 2000. A correlational test of the relationship between posttraumatic growth, religion, and cognitive processing. Journal of Trauma and Stress 13: 521–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pargament, K. I., Koenig, H. G, Tarakeshwar, N et al. 2004. Religious coping methods as predictors of psychological, physical and spiritual outcomes among medically ill elderly patients: a two-year longitudinal study. Journal of Health Psychology 9: 713–30.Google Scholar
Shaw, A., Joseph, S and Linley, P. A.. 2005. Religion, spirituality, and posttraumatic growth: a systematic review. Mental Health, Religion, and Culture 8: 111.Google Scholar
Dezutter, J., Wachholtz, A and Corveleyn, J. 2011. Prayer and pain: the mediating role of positive re-appraisal. Journal of Behavioral Medicine 34: 542–9.Google Scholar
Bussing, A., Michalsen, A, Balzat, H. J et al. 2009. Are spirituality and religiosity resources for patients with chronic pain conditions? Pain Medicine 10: 327–39.Google Scholar
Wiech, K., Farias, M, Kahane, G et al. 2008. An fMRI study measuring analgesia enhanced by religion as a belief system. Pain 139: 467–76.Google Scholar
Charlton, G. S., and Barrow, C. J.. 2002. Coping and self-help group membership in Parkinson’s disease: an exploratory qualitative study. Health and Social Care in the Community 10: 472–8.Google Scholar
de Vito, S., Gamboz, N, Brandimonte, M. A et al. 2012. Future thinking in Parkinson’s disease: an executive function? Neuropsychologia 50: 14941501.Google Scholar
Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jorgensen, H, Geertz, A. W and Roepstorff, A. 2009. Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4: 199207.Google Scholar
Kapogiannis, D., Barbey, A. K, Su, M et al. 2009. Neuroanatomical variability of religiosity. PLoS One 4, no. 9: e7180. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poletti, M, Enrici, I and Adenzato, M. 2012. Cognitive and affective theory of mind in neurodegenerative diseases: neuropsychological, neuroanatomical and neurochemical levels. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 36: 2147–64.Google Scholar
Cummins, G. 2012. Social cognition in Parkinson’s disease and other movement disorders. Unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Cambridge.Google Scholar
Nombela, C., Bustillo, P. J., Castell, P. F. et al. 2011. Cognitive rehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease: evidence from neuroimaging. Frontiers in Neurology 2: 82.Google Scholar
McNamara, P. 2009. The Neuroscience of Religious Experience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Previc, F. H. 2006. The role of the extrapersonal brain systems in religious activity. Consciousness and Cognition 15: 500–39.Google Scholar
Liddle, P. F., Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D. et al. 1992. Patterns of cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry 160: 179–86.Google Scholar
Schultz, W. 2002. Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron 36: 241–63.Google Scholar
Cools, R. 2011. Dopaminergic control of the striatum for high-level cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 21: 402–7.Google Scholar
Valeo, T. 2012. Not just tremor: recognizing depression and other non-motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease. Neurology Now Magazine 8: 22–7.Google Scholar
McNamara, P., Durso, R. and Brown, A.. 2006. Religiosity in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychiatric Disease Treatment 2: 341–8.Google Scholar
Butler, P. M., McNamara, P. and Durso, R.. 2010. Deficits in the automatic activation of religious concepts in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 16: 252–61.Google Scholar
Butler, P. M., McNamara, P., Ghofrani, J et al. 2011. Disease-associated differences in religious cognition in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 33: 917–28.Google Scholar
Giaquinto, S., Bruti, L, Dall’Armi, V et al. 2011. Religious and spiritual beliefs in outpatients suffering from Parkinson disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 26: 916–22.Google Scholar
Butler, P. M., McNamara, P and Durso, R. 2011. Side of onset in Parkinson’s disease and alterations in religiosity: novel behavioral phenotypes. Behavioral Neurology 24: 133–41.Google Scholar
Kousta, S. T., Vigliocco, G, Vinson, D. P. et al. 2011. The representation of abstract words: why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology – General 140: 1434.Google Scholar
Schwanenflugel, P. J., Harnishfeger, K. K. and Stowe, R. W.. 1988. Context availability and lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words. Journal of Memory and Language 27: 499520.Google Scholar
Donald, M. 2004. How Culture and Brain Mechanisms Interact in Decision Making. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar

References

Asp, E. W., and Tranel, D.. 2013. False tagging theory: toward a unitary account of prefrontal cortex function. In Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, 2nd edn., ed. Stuss, D. T. and Knight, R. T., 383416. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Asp, E. W., Manzel, K., Koestner, B., Cole, C. A., Denburg, N. L. and Tranel, D.. 2012a. A neuropsychological test of belief and doubt: damage to ventromedial prefrontal cortex increases credulity for misleading advertising. Frontiers in Neuroscience 6: 100–9.Google Scholar
Asp, E. W., Ramchandran, K. and Tranel, D.. 2012b. Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, and the human prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychology 26: 414–21.Google Scholar
Asp, E. W., Manzel, K., Koestner, B., Denburg, N. L. and Tranel, D.. 2013. Benefit of the doubt: a new view of the role of the prefrontal cortex in executive functioning and decision making. Frontiers in Neuroscience 7: 86.Google Scholar
Asp, E. W., Warner, K. A., Denburg, N. L. and Tranel, D.. N.d. Credulity and religious beliefs in patients with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex damage. Department of Neurology, University of Iowa.Google Scholar
Atran, S., and Norenzayan, A.. 2004. Religion’s evolutionary landscape: counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 27: 713–70.Google Scholar
Barbey, A. K., and Sloman, S. A.. 2007. Base-rate respect: from ecological rationality to dual processes. Behavioural and Brain Sciences 30: 241–56.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. L., and Keil, F. C.. 1996. Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: anthropomorphism in god concepts. Cognitive Psychology 31: 219–47.Google Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S. and Cohen, J. D.. 2001. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychological Review 108: 624–52.Google Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D. and Carter, C. S.. 2004. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 539–46.Google Scholar
Browne, M., Pennycook, G., Goodwin, B. and McHenry, M.. 2014. Reflective minds and open hearts: cognitive style and personality predict religious and spiritual thinking in a community sample. European Journal of Social Psychology 44: 736–42.Google Scholar
Cheyne, J. A., and Pennycook, G.. 2013. Sleep paralysis postepisode distress: modeling potential effects of episode characteristics, general psychological distress, beliefs, and cognitive style. Clinical Psychological Science 1: 135–48.Google Scholar
De Neys, W. 2014. Conflict detection, dual processes, and logical intuitions: Some clarifications. Thinking and Reasoning 20: 167–87.Google Scholar
De Neys, W., Vartanian, O. and Goel, V.. 2008. When our brains detect that we are biased. Psychological Science 19: 483–9.Google Scholar
Duncan, J., and Miller, E. K.. 2002. Cognitive focus through adaptive neural coding in the primate prefrontal cortex. In Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, ed. Stuss, D. T. and Knight, R. T., 278–91. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Egan, A. 2008. Seeing and believing: perception, belief formation and the divided mind. Philosophical Studies 140: 4763.Google Scholar
Evans, J. St. B. T., and Stanovich, K. E.. 2013. Dual-process theories of higher cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives in Psychological Science 8: 223–41.Google Scholar
Gallup, . 2011. U.S. political ideology stable with conservatives leading. August 1. www.gallup.com/poll/148745/political-ideology-stable-conservatives-leading.aspx.Google Scholar
Gehring, W. J., and Knight, R. T.. 2000. Prefrontal-cingulate interations in action monitoring. Nature Neuroscience 3: 516–20.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M., and Norenzayan, A.. 2012. Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science 336: 493–6.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. 1991. How mental systems believe. American Psychologist 46: 107–19.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T. 1993. The assent of man: mental representation and the control of belief. In Handbook of Mental Control, ed. Wegner, D. M. and Pennebaker, J. W., 5787. Engelwood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., Tafarodi, R. W. and Malone, P. S.. 1993. You can’t not believe everything you read. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65: 221–33.Google Scholar
Glascher, J., Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Bechara, A., Rudrauf, D., Calamia, M. et al. 2012. Lesion mapping of cognitive control and value-based decision making in the prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109: 14681–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guthrie, S. E. 1993. Faces in the Clouds: A New Theory of Religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hasson, U., Simmons, J. P. and Todorov, A.. 2005. Believe it or not. Psychological Science 16: 566–71.Google Scholar
Hunsberger, B., McKenzie, B., Pratt, M. and Pancer, S. M.. 1993. Religious doubt: a social-psychological analysis. Research in the Social Scientific Study of Religion 5: 2751.Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M., McGregor, I., Hirsh, J. B. and Nash, K.. 2009. Neural markers of religious conviction. Psychological Science 20: 385–92.Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M., Tullett, A. M. and Good, M.. 2011. The need to believe: a neuroscience account of religion as a motivated process. Religion, Brain, and Behavior 1: 192212.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A.. 1973. On the psychology of prediction. Psychological Review 80: 237–51.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D. 2004. Are children ‘intuitive theists’? Psychological Science 15: 295301.Google Scholar
Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., Cho, R. Y., Stenger, V. A. and Carter, C. S.. 2004. Anterior cingulate conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science 303: 1023–6.Google Scholar
Mandelbaum, E. 2014. Thinking is believing. Inquiry 57: 5596.Google Scholar
Muramoto, O. 2004. The role of the medial prefrontal cortex in human religious activity. Medical Hypotheses 62: 479–85.Google Scholar
Nadarevic, L., and Erdfelder, E.. 2013. Spinoza’s error: memory for truth and falsity. Memory and Cognition 41: 176–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pennycook, G. 2014. Evidence that analytic cognitive style influences religious belief: comment on Razmyar and Reeve (2013). Intelligence 43: 21–6.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Seli, P., Koehler, D. J. and Fugelsang, J. A.. 2012. Analytic cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition 123: 335–46.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Koehler, D. J. and Fugelsang, J. A.. 2013. Belief bias during reasoning among religious believers and skeptics. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 20: 806–11.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J. and Fugelsang, J. A.. 2014a. Cognitive style and religiosity: the role of conflict detection. Memory and Cognition 42: 110.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J. and Fugelsang, J. A.. 2014b. The role of analytic thinking in moral judgements and values. Thinking and Reasoning 20: 188214.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Trippas, D., Handley, S. J. and Thompson, V. A.. 2014c. Base-rates: both neglected and intuitive. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 40: 544–54.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Fugelsang, J. A. and Koehler, D. J.. 2015. What makes us think? A three-stage dual-process model of analytic engagement. Cognitive Psychology 80: 3472.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Ross, R., Koehler, D. J. and Fugelsang, J. A.. 2016. Atheists and agnostics are more reflective than religious believers: four empirical studies and a meta-analysis. PLoS One 11: e0153039.Google Scholar
Razmyar, S., and Reeve, C. L.. 2013. Individual differences in religiosity as a function of cognitive ability and style. Intelligence 41: 667–73.Google Scholar
Shackman, A. J., Salomons, T. V., Slagter, H. A., Fox, A. S., Winter, J. J. and Davidson, R. J.. 2011. The integration of negative affect, pain and cogntive control in the cingulate cortex. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 12: 154–67.Google Scholar
Shenhav, A., Rand, D. G. and Greene, J. D.. 2012. Divine intuition: cognitive style influences belief in god. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 141: 423–8.Google Scholar
Shreve-Neiger, A. K., and Edelstein, B. A.. 2004. Religion and anxiety: a critical review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review 24: 379–97.Google Scholar
Spelke, E., Phillips, A. and Woodward, A.. 1995. Infants’ knowledge of object motion and human action. In Causal Cognition, ed. Sperber, D., Premack, D. and Premack, A., 4478. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Stanovich, K. E., and West, R. F.. 2003. Evolutionary versus instrumental goals: how evolutionary psychology misconceives human rationality. In Evolution and the Psychology of Thinking: The Debate, ed. Over, D. E., 153. New York: Psychological Press.Google Scholar
Svedholm, A. M., and Lindeman, M.. 2012. The separate roles of the reflective mind and involuntary inhibitory control in gatekeeping paranormal beliefs and the underlying intuitive confusions. British Journal of Psychology 104: 303–19.Google Scholar
Van Leeuwen, N. 2014. Religious credence is not factual belief. Cognition 13: 698715.Google Scholar
Yonker, J. E., Edman, L. R. O., Cresswell, J. and Barrett, J. L.. 2016. Primed analytic thought and religiosity: the importance of individual characteristics. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality 8: 298308.Google Scholar
Zuckerman, P. 2007. Atheism: contemporary numbers and patterns. In The Cambridge Companion to Atheism, ed. Martin, M., 4765. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

References

Kerndt, P. R., Naughton, J. L., Driscoll, C. E and Loxterkamp, D. A.. 1982. Fasting: the history, pathophysiology and complications. Western Journal of Medicine 137: 379–99.Google Scholar
Quran: Al-Baqarah, verse 183.Google Scholar
Bukhari and Muslim and others.Google Scholar
Sakr, A. H. 1975. Fasting in Islam. Journal of the American Dietetic Association 67: 1721.Google Scholar
Sahih Bukhari vol. 3, no. 178.Google Scholar
Sahih Bukhari vol. 3, no. 146.Google Scholar
Sahih Muslim 1096 a, Book 13, Hadith 56. USC-MSA web (English) reference: Book 6, Hadith 2413.Google Scholar
Baianonie, Mohamed, imam of the Islamic Center of Raleigh, NC, www.raleighmasjid.org/imam/fiqh-ramadan.html.Google Scholar
Aadil, N., Houti, I. E. and Moussamih, S.. 2004. Drug intake during Ramadan. British Medical Journal 329: 778–82.Google Scholar
Aslam, M., and Healy, M. A.. 1986. Compliance and drug therapy in fasting Moslem patients. Journal of Clinical and Hospital Pharmacy 11: 321–5.Google Scholar
Aslam, M., and Assad, A.. 1986. Drug regimens and fasting during Ramadan: a survey in KuwaitPublic Health 100: 4953.Google Scholar
Zeeneldin, A. A., Gaber, A. A. and Taha, F. M.. 2012. Does fasting during Ramadan trigger non-adherence to oral hormonal therapy in breast cancer patients? Journal of the Egyptian National Cancer Institute 24: 133–7.Google Scholar
Recommendations of the 9th Fiqh-Medical seminar, ‘An Islamic View of Certain Contemporary Medical Issues’, Casablanca, Morocco, 14–17 June 1997, www.islamset.com/search/index.html.Google Scholar
Kumar, N., and Jivan, S.. 2007. Ramadan and eyedrops: the Muslim perspective. Ophthalmology 114: 2356–60.Google Scholar
Kumar, N., Jivan, S. and Buchan, J.. 2008. Compliance with prescribed treatment during Ramadan. Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology 36: 699.Google Scholar
Patel, T., Magdum, A. and Ghura, V.. 2012. Does fasting during Ramadan affect the use of topical dermatological treatment by Muslim patients in the UK? Clinical and Experimental Dermatology 37: 718–21.Google Scholar
Zabeen, B., Tayyeb, S., Benarjee, B, Baki, A, Nahar, J, Mohsin, F, Nahar, N and Azad, K.. 2014. Fasting during Ramadan in adolescents with diabetes. Indian Journal of Endocrinology and Metabolism 18: 44–7.Google Scholar
Bener, A., Hamad, A., Fares, A, Al-Sayed, H. M and Al-Suwaidi, J.. 2006. Is there any effect of Ramadan fasting on stroke incidence? Singapore Medical Journal 47: 404–8.Google Scholar
Akhan, G., Kutluhan, S. and Koyuncuoglu, H. R.. 2000. Is there any change of stroke incidence during Ramadan? Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 101: 259–61.Google Scholar
El-Mitwalli, A., Zaher, A. A., Mohamed, M. A and Elmenshawi, E.. 2009. The effect of Ramadan fasting on cerebral stroke: a prospective hospital-based study. European Journal of Neurology 16, no. 4: e80.Google Scholar
El-Mitwalli, A., Zaher, A. A. and El Menshawi, E.. 2010. Circadian rhythm of stroke onset during the month of Ramadan. Acta Neurologica Scandinavica 122: 97101.Google Scholar
Saadatnia, M., Zare, M., Fatehi, F and Ahmadi, A.. 2009. The effect of fasting on cerebral venous and dural sinus thrombosis. Neurology Research 31: 794–8.Google Scholar
Burazeri, G., Goda, A. and Kark, J. D.. 2008. Religious observance and acute coronary syndrome in predominantly Muslim Albania: a population-based case-control study in Tirana. Annals of Epidemiology 18: 937–45.Google Scholar
Nakhaee, N., Divsalar, K. and Jadidi, N.. 2009. Religious involvement and cigarette smoking among Iranian university students. International Journal of Psychiatry and Medicine 39: 189–98.Google Scholar
Nematy, M., Alinezhad-Namaghi, M., Rashed, M. M, Mozhdehifard, M, Sajjadi, S. S, Akhlaghi, S, Sabery, M, Mohajeri, S. A, Shalaey, N, Moohebati, M and Norouzy, A.. 2012. Effects of Ramadan fasting on cardiovascular risk factors: a prospective observational study. Nutrition Journal 11: 69.Google Scholar
Saleh, S. A., El-Kemery, T. A., Farrag, K. A, Badawy, M. R, Sarkis, N. N, Soliman, F. S and Mangoud, H.. 2004. Ramadan fasting: relation to atherogenic risk among obese Muslims. Journal of the Egyptian Public Health Association 79: 461–83.Google Scholar
Norouzy, A., Salehi, M., Philippou, E, Arabi, H, Shiva, F, Mehrnoosh, S, Mohajeri, S. M, Mohajeri, S. A, Motaghedi Larijani, A and Nematy, M.. 2013. Effect of fasting in Ramadan on body composition and nutritional intake: a prospective study. Journal of Human Nutrition and Diet 26, Suppl. 1: 97104.Google Scholar
Sarraf-Zadegan, N., Atashi, M., Naderi, G. A, Baghai, A. M, Asgary, S, Fatehifar, M. R, Samarian, H and Zarei, M.. 2000. The effect of fasting in Ramadan on the values and interrelations between biochemical, coagulation and hematological factors. Annals of Saudi Medicine 20: 377–81.Google Scholar
Qujeq, D., Bijani, K., Kalavi, K, Mohiti, J and Aliakbarpour, H.. 2002. Effects of Ramadan fasting on serum low-density and high-density lipoprotein-cholesterol concentrations. Annals of Saudi Medicine 22: 297–9.Google Scholar
Afrasiabi, A., Hassanzadeh, S., Sattarivand, R and Mahboob, S.. 2003. Effects of Ramadan fasting on serum lipid profiles on 2 hyperlipidemic groups with or without diet pattern. Saudi Medical Journal 24: 23–6.Google Scholar
Shehab, A., Abdulle, A., El Issa, A, Al Suwaidi, J and Nagelkerke, N.. 2012. Favorable changes in lipid profile: the effects of fasting after Ramadan. PLoS One 7, no. 10: e47615.Google Scholar
Dalton, K. 1975. Food intake prior to a migraine attack-study of 2313 spontaneous attack. Headache 15: 188–93.Google Scholar
Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Classification. 1988. Classification and diagnostic criteria for headache disorders, cranial neuralgias and facial pain. Cephalalgia 8, Suppl. 7: 196.Google Scholar
Headache Classification Subcommittee of the International Headache Society. 2004. The International Classification of Headache Disorders. Cephalalgia 24, Suppl. 1: 1160.Google Scholar
Headache Classification Committee of the International Headache Society. 2013. The International Classification of Headache Disorders, 3rd edition (beta version). Cephalalgia 33, no. 9: 629808.Google Scholar
Mosek, A., and Korczyn, A. D.. 1995. The Yom Kippur headache. Neurology 45: 1953–5.Google Scholar
Mosek, A., and Korczyn, A. D.. 1999. Fasting headache, weight loss, and dehydration. Headache 39: 225–7.Google Scholar
Abu-Salameh, I., Plakht, Y. and Ifergane, G.. 2010. Migraine exacerbation during Ramadan fasting. Journal of Headache Pain 11: 513–17.Google Scholar
Drescher, M. J., Wimpfheimer, Z., Abu Khalef, S., Gammaitoni, A, Shehadeh, N and Torgovicky, R.. 2012. Prophylactic etoricoxib is effective in preventing ‘first of Ramadan’ headache: a placebo-controlled double-blind and randomized trial of prophylactic etoricoxib for ritual fasting headache. Headache 52: 573–81.Google Scholar
Guelpa, G. M. A. 1911. La lutte contre l’epilepsie par la desintoxication et par la reducation alimentaire. Revue Therapeutique Medica-Chirugie 78: 813.Google Scholar
Geyelin, H. R. 1921. Fasting as a method for treating epilepsy. Medical Record 99: 1037–9.Google Scholar
Conklin, H. W. 1922. Cause and treatment of epilepsy. American Journal of the Osteopathic Association 26: 1114.Google Scholar
Hartman, A. L., Rubenstein, J. E. and Kossoff, E. H.. 2013. Intermittent fasting: a ‘new’ historical strategy for controlling seizures? Epilepsy Research 104: 275–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gomceli, Y. B., Kutlu, G., Cavdar, L and Inan, L. E.. 2008. Does the seizure frequency increase in Ramadan? Seizure 17: 671–6.Google Scholar
Al-Qattan, M. M. 2000. Burns in epileptics in Saudi Arabia. Burns 26: 561–3.Google Scholar
Aadil, N., Fassi-Fihri, A., Houti, I, Benaji, B, Ouhakki, M, Kotbi, S, Diquet, B and Hakkou, F.. 2000. Influence of Ramadan on the pharmacokinetics of a single oral dose of valproic acid administered at two different times. Methods and Findings in Experimental and Clinical Pharmacology 22: 109–14.Google Scholar
Khattab, E., Mahmood, I. H. and Abdul-Jabbar, E. T.. 2007. Can epileptic patients on carbamazepine safely fast Ramadan? JIMA 38486: 156–60.Google Scholar
Latifynia, A., Vojgani, M., Abofazeli, T and Jafarieh, H.. 2007. Circulating immune complex during Ramadan. Journal of Ayub Medical College Abbottabad 19, no. 2: 1518.Google Scholar
Faris, M. A., Kacimi, S., Al-Kurd, R. A, Fararjeh, M. A, Bustanji, Y. K, Mohammad, M. K and Salem, M. L.. 2012. Intermittent fasting during Ramadan attenuates proinflammatory cytokines and immune cells in healthy subjects. Nutrition Research 32: 947–55.Google Scholar
Latifynia, A., Vojgani, M., Gharagozlou, M. J and Sharifian, R.. 2009. Neutrophil function (innate immunity) during Ramadan. Journal of Ayub Medical College Abbottabad 21, no. 4: 111–15.Google Scholar
Saadatnia, M., Etemadifar, M., Fatehi, F, Ashtari, F, Shaygannejad, V, Chitsaz, A and Maghzi, A. H.. 2009. Short-term effects of prolonged fasting on multiple sclerosis. European Neurology 61: 230–2.Google Scholar
Abdeldayem, S. M., and Hossam Zyton, A. H.. 2012. The effect of Ramadan fasting on multiple sclerosis. Egyptian Journal of Neurology, Psychiatry, and Neurosurgery 494: 341–5.Google Scholar
Jahromi, S. R., Sahraian, M. A., Ashtari, F, Ayromlou, H, Etemadifar, M, Ghaffarpour, M, Mohammadianinejad, E, Nafissi, S, Nickseresht, A, Shaygannejad, V, Togha, M, Torabi, H. R and Ziaie, S.. 2014. Islamic fasting and multiple sclerosis. BMC Neurology 14: 56.Google Scholar
Bahammam, A., Alrajeh, M., Albabtain, M, Bahammam, S and Sharif, M.. 2010. Circadian pattern of sleep, energy expenditure, and body temperature of young healthy men during the intermittent fasting of Ramadan. Appetite 54: 426–9.Google Scholar
Margolis, S. A., and Reed, R. L.. 2004. Effect of religious practices of Ramadan on sleep and perceived sleepiness of medical students. Teaching and Learning in Medicine 16: 145–9.Google Scholar
Bahammam, A. S., Nashwan, S., Hammad, O, Sharif, M. M and Pandi-Perumal, S. R.. 2013. Objective assessment of drowsiness and reaction time during intermittent Ramadan fasting in young men: a case-crossover study. Behavioral and Brain Functions 9: 32.Google Scholar
Bahammam, A. S., Almushailhi, K., Pandi-Perumal, S. R and Sharif, M. M.. 2014. Intermittent fasting during Ramadan: does it affect sleep? Journal of Sleep Research 23: 3543.Google Scholar
Dolu, N., Yüksek, A., Sizer, A and Alay, M.. 2007. Arousal and continuous attention during Ramadan intermittent fasting. Journal of Basic and Clinical Physiology and Pharmacology 18: 315–22.Google Scholar
Sahih Bukhari vol. 3, no. 127.Google Scholar
Herrag, M., Lahmiti, S. and Yazidi, A. A.. 2010. Ramadan: a different side of the emergencies? African Health Sciences 10: 215–16.Google Scholar
Koushali, A. N., Hajiamini, Z., Ebadi, A, Bayat, N and Khamseh, F.. 2013. Effect of Ramadan fasting on emotional reactions in nurses. Iranian Journal of Nursing and Midwifery Research 18: 232–6.Google Scholar

References

American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistic Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.Google Scholar
Attwood, T. 2007. The Complete Guide to Asperger’s Syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. 1995. Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. 1999. Theological correctness: Cognitive constraint and the study of religion. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 11: 325–39.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. 2004. Why Would Anyone Believe in God? Lanham, MD: AltaMira.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. 2012. Born Believers: The Science of Children’s Religious Beliefs. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Bering, J. 2002. The existential theory of mind. Review of General Psychology 6: 324.Google Scholar
Bird, G., and Cook, R.. 2013. Mixed emotions: the contribution of alexithymia to emotional symptoms of autism. Translational Psychiatry 3: 18.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. 2007. Religion is natural. Developmental Science 10: 147–51.Google Scholar
Brock, J., Brown, C. B., Boucher, J. and Rippon, G.. 2002. The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism. Development and Psychopathology 14: 209–24.Google Scholar
Caldwell-Harris, C. 2012. Understanding atheism/non-belief as an expected individual-differences variable. Religion, Brain, and Behavior 2: 447.Google Scholar
Calver, A. 2009. The unicorn who came to stay and other stories: mothers’ experiences of having a child with autism with an imaginary friend. Doctoral dissertation, University of London.Google Scholar
Clark, K. J. 2019. God and the Brain. Grand Rapids, MI: Eerdmans.Google Scholar
Clark, K. J., and Barrett, J.. 2010. Reformed epistemology and the cognitive science of religion. Faith and Philosophy 27: 174–89.Google Scholar
Davidson, J., and Orsini, M.. 2013. The shifting horizons of autism online. In Worlds of Autism: Across the Spectrum of Neurological Difference, ed. Michael, Davidson Joyce Orsini, 285303. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Davis, P. E., Simon, H., Meins, E. and Robin, D. L.. 2018. Imaginary companions in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 48: 110. http://csjarchive.cogsci.rpi.edu/proceedings/2011/papers/0782/paper0782.pdf.Google Scholar
Dubin, N., and Graetz, J. E.. 2009. Through a different lens: spirituality in the lives of individuals with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Religion, Disability, and Health 13: 2939.Google Scholar
Farias, M., van Mulukom, V., Kahane, G., Kreplin, U., Joyce, A., Soares, P., Oviedo, L., Hernu, M., Rokita, K., Savulescu, J., Mottonen, R.. 2017. Supernatural belief is not modulated by intuitive thinking style or cognitive inhibition. Scientific Reports. 7. 10.1038/s41598-017-14090-9.Google Scholar
Foss-Feig, J. H., Kwakye, L. D., Cascio, C. J., Burnette, C. P., Kadivar, H., Stone, W. L. and Wallace, M. T.. 2010. An extended multisensory temporal binding window in autism spectrum disorders. Experimental Brain Research 203: 381–9.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M., and Norenzayan, A.. 2012. Analytic thinking promotes religious disbeliefScience 336: 493–6.Google Scholar
Gervais, W. M., Van Elk, M., Xygalatas, D., McKay, R., Aveyard, M., Buchtel, E., Dar-Nimrod, I., Kundtová Klocová, E., Ramsay, J., Riekki, T., Svedholm-Häkkinen, A., Bulbulia, J.. 2018. Analytic atheism: A cross-culturally weak and fickle phenomenon? Judgment and Decision Making 13, no. 3, May 2018: 268–74.Google Scholar
Guthrie, S. 1993. Faces in the Clouds: A New Theory of Religion. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heywood, B. T. 2010. Meant to be: how religious beliefs, cultural religiosity, and impaired theory of mind affect the implicit bias to think teleologically. Doctoral dissertation, Queens University, Belfast.Google Scholar
Heywood, B. T., and Bering, J. M.. 2013. Meant to be: how religious beliefs and cultural religiosity affect the implicit bias to think teleologically. Religion, Brain, and Behavior 41: 183201.Google Scholar
Jack, A. I., Friedman, J. P., Boyatzis, R. E. and Taylor, S. N.. 2016. Why do you believe in God? Relationships between religious belief, analytic thinking, mentalizing and moral concern. PloS ONE 11, no. 3: e0149989.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D., and Rosset, E.. 2009. The human function compunction: teleological explanation in adults. Cognition 111: 138–43.Google Scholar
Kirkpatrick, L. A. 2013. Evolutionary psychology as a foundation for the psychology of religion. In The Psychology of Religion: A Multidisciplinary Approach, ed. Paloutzian, R. and Park, C., 118–37. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lindeman, M., and Lipsanen, J.. 2016. Diverse cognitive profiles of religious believers and nonbelievers. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 26, no. 3: 18.Google Scholar
Luhrmann, T. 2012. When God Talks Back: Understanding the American, Evangelical Relationship with God. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Maij, D. L. R., van Harreveld, F., Gervais, W., Schrag, Y., Mohr, C. and van Elk, M.. 2017. Mentalizing skills do not differentiate believers from non-believers but credibility enhancing displays do. PLoS ONE 12, no. 8: e0182764.Google Scholar
McCauley, R. N. 2012. Why Religion Is Natural and Science Is Not. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Noranzayan, A., and Gervais, W. M.. 2013. The origins of religious disbelief. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17: 20–5.Google Scholar
Pennycook, G., Ross, R. M., Koehler, D. J. and Fugelsang, J. A.. 2016. Atheists and agnostics are more reflective than religious believers: four empirical studies and a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 11: 118.Google Scholar
Reddish, P., Tok, P. and Kundt, R.. 2016. Religious cognition and behaviour in autism: the role of mentalizing. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 26: 95112.Google Scholar
Sanchez, S., Sundermeier, B., Gray, K. and Calin-Jagemanl, R.. 2017. Direct replication of Gervais & Norenzayan (2012): no evidence that analytic thinking decreases religious belief. Plos ONE 12, no. 2: e0172636.Google Scholar
Smith, A. 2009. Emotional empathy in autism spectrum conditions: Weak, intact, or heightened? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 39: 1747–8.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. 1997. Intuitive and reflected beliefs. Mind and Language 12: 6783.Google Scholar
Tremlin, T. 2005. Divergent religion: a dual-process model of religious thought, behavior, and morphology. In Mind and Religion: Psychological and Cognitive Foundations of Religiosity, ed. Whitehouse, H. and McCauley, R. N., 6984. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Tremlin, T. 2006. Minds and Gods: The Cognitive Foundations of Religion. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Visuri, I. 2012. Could everyone talk to God? A case study on Asperger’s syndrome, religion and spirituality. Journal of Religion, Health, and Disability 16: 352–78.Google Scholar
Visuri, I. 2018. Rethinking autism, theism and atheism: bodiless agents and imaginary realities. Archive for the Psychology of Religion 1: 131.Google Scholar
Visuri, I. 2019. Sensory supernatural experiences in autism. Religion, Brain, and Behavior. Published online. doi:10.1080/2153599X.2018.1548374.Google Scholar
Visuri, I. 2019, in press. A room of one’s own: autistic imagination as a stage for parasocial interaction and social learning. Journal for the Cognitive Science of Religion.Google Scholar
Wigger, J. B., Paxson, K. and Ryan, L.. 2013. What do invisible friends know? Imaginary companions, God, and theory of mind. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 23: 214.Google Scholar
Winnicott, D. 1971. Playing and Reality. London: Tavistock.Google Scholar

References

Hughes, J. C. 2011. Alzheimer’s and Other Dementias: The Facts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ames, D., Burns, A. and O’Brien, J., eds. 2010. Dementia. 4th edn. London: Hodder Arnold.Google Scholar
Hughes, J. C. 2011. Thinking through Dementia. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hughes, J. C. 2013. Dementia is dead, long live ageing: philosophy and practice in connection with ‘dementia’. In Oxford Handbook of Philosophy and Psychiatry, eds. Fulford, K. W. M., Davies, M., Gipps, R. G. T., Graham, G., Sadler, J. Z., Stanghellini, G. and Thornton, T., 835–50. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sachdev, P. S. 2013. The DSM-5 approach to dementia. In Oxford Textbook of Old Age Psychiatry, 2nd edn., ed. Dening, T. and Thomas, A., 377–81. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Snowdon, D. A. 2003. Healthy aging and dementia: findings from the Nun Study. Annals of Internal Medicine 139: 450–4.Google Scholar
Miller, B. L., Cummings, J., Mishkin, F., Boone, K., Prince, F., Ponton, M. and Cotman, C.. 1998. Emergence of artistic talent in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 51: 978–82.Google Scholar
Wuerfel, J., Krishnamoorthy, E. S., Brown, R. J., Lemieux, L., Koepp, M., van Elst, L. Tebartz and Trimble, M. R.. 2004. Religiosity is associated with hippocampal but not amygdala volumes in patients with refractory epilepsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 75: 640–2.Google Scholar
Chan, D., Anderson, V., Pijnenburg, Y., Whitwell, J., Barnes, J, Scahill, R, Stevens, J. M., Barkhof, F, Scheltens, P, Rossor, M. N. and Fox, N. C.. 2009. The clinical profile of right temporal lobe atrophy. Brain 132: 1287–9.Google Scholar
Postiglione, A., Milan, G., Pappatà, S, De Falco, C, Lamenza, F, Schiattarella, V, Gallotta, G, Sorrentino, P and Striano, S.. 2008. Fronto-temporal dementia presenting as Geschwind’s syndrome. Neurocase 14: 264–70.Google Scholar
Lombrozo, T., Kelemen, D. and Zaitchik, D.. 2007. Inferring design: evidence of a preference for teleological explanations in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Psychological Science 18: 9991006.Google Scholar
Moberg, D. O. 2005. Research in spirituality, religion, and aging. Journal of Gerontology and Social Work 45: 1140.Google Scholar
Stuckey, J. C., Post, S. G., Ollerton, S, Fallcreek, S. J. and Whitehouse, P. J.. 2002. Alzheimer’s disease, religion, and the ethics of respect for spirituality: a community dialogue. Alzheimer’s Care Quarterly 3: 199207.Google Scholar
Lawrence, R. M. 2003. Aspects of spirituality in dementia care: when clinicians tune into silence. Dementia 2: 393402.Google Scholar
Jolley, D., Benbow, S. M., Grizzell, M., Willmott, S, Bawn, S and Kingston, P.. 2010. Spirituality and faith in dementia. Dementia 9: 311–25.Google Scholar
Lawrence, R. M. 2007. Dementia: a personal legacy beyond word. Mental Health Religion and Culture 10: 553–62.Google Scholar
Goodall, M. A. 2009. The evaluation of spiritual care in a dementia care setting. Dementia 8: 167–83.Google Scholar
Jolley, D., and Moreland, N.. 2011. Dementia care: spiritual and faith perspectives. Nursing and Residential Care 13: 388–91.Google Scholar
Keast, K., Leskovar, C. and Brohm, R.. 2010. A systematic review of spirituality and dementia in LTC. Annals of Long-Term Care 18: 41–7.Google Scholar
Davis, R., and Davis, B.. 1989. My Journey into Alzheimer’s Disease. Wheaton, IL: Tyndale House.Google Scholar
Normann, H. K., Asplund, K. and Norberg, A.. 1998. Episodes of lucidity in people with severe dementia as narrated by formal carers. Journal of Advanced Nursing 28: 1295300.Google Scholar
Franzen, J. 2001. The long slow slide into the abyss. Guardian Weekend, 15 December, 15–29.Google Scholar
Franzen, J. 2001. My father’s brain: what Alzheimer’s takes away. New Yorker, 10 September, 81–91.Google Scholar
Ball, A. 2006. Ellen’s prayer. In Palliative Care in Severe Dementia, ed. Hughes, J. C., 134. London: Quay Books.Google Scholar
Sabat, S. R., and Harré, R.. 1994. The Alzheimer’s disease sufferer as a semiotic subject. Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology 1: 145–60.Google Scholar
Feil, N. 1993. The Validation Breakthrough: Simple Techniques for Communicating with People with ‘Alzheimer’s Type Dementia’. Baltimore: Health Professions Press.Google Scholar
Allan, K., and Killick, J.. 2010. Communication with people with dementia. In Supportive Care for the Person with Dementia, ed. Hughes, J. C., Lloyd-Williams, M. and Sachs, G. A., 217–25. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Widdershoven, G. A. M., and Berghmans, R. L. P.. 2006. Meaning-making in dementia: a hermeneutic perspective. In Dementia: Mind, Meaning, and the Person, ed. Hughes, J. C., Louw, S. J. and Sabat, S. R., 179–91. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sapp, S. 2010. Spiritual care of people with dementia and their carers. In Supportive Care for the Person with Dementia, eds. Hughes, J. C., Lloyd-Williams, M. and Sachs, G. A., 199206. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Collicutt, J. 2012. Some ethical issues in dementia care: re-membering the person. Crucible, October–December, 7–17.Google Scholar
Allen, F. B., and Coleman, P. G.. 2006. Spiritual perspectives on the person with dementia: identity and personhood. In Dementia: Mind, Meaning, and the Person, ed. Hughes, J. C., Louw, S. J. and Sabat, S. R., 205–21. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hughes, J. C. 2001. Views of the person with dementia. Journal of Medical Ethics 27: 8691.Google Scholar
Hughes, J. C. 2011. A situated embodied view of the person with dementia: where does the spiritual come in? In Spirituality and Personhood in Dementia, ed. Jewell, A., 198206. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
McCabe, H. 1987. God Matters. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
Pointon, B. 2007. ‘Who am I? – The search for spirituality in dementia. A family carer’s perspective. In Spirituality, Values and Mental Health: Jewels for the Journey, ed. Coyte, M. E., Gilbert, P. and Nicholls, V., 114–20. London: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. 1968. Philosophical Investigations. Ed. Anscombe, G. E. M. and Rhees, R.. Trans. Anscombe, G. E. M.. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Nuffield Council on Bioethics. 2009. Dementia: Ethical Issues. London: Nuffield Council on Bioethics. www.nuffieldbioethics.org/dementia.Google Scholar

References

Jaynes, J. 1976. The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Park, C. L. 2005. Religion as a meaning making framework in coping with life stress. Journal of Social Issues 61: 707–29.Google Scholar
Geschwind, N. 1983. Pathogenesis of behavior change in temporal lobe epilepsy. Research Publications – Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disorders 61: 355–70.Google Scholar
Bear, D. M., and Fedio, P.. 1977. Quantitative analysis of interictal behavior in temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives in Neurology 34: 454–67.Google Scholar
Devinsky, O., and Lai, G.. 2008. Spirituality and religion in epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior 12: 636–43.Google Scholar
Trimble, M., and Freeman, A.. 2006. An investigation of religiosity and the Gastaut-Geschwind syndrome in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behavior 9: 407–14.Google Scholar
Edwards-Lee, T., Miller, B. L., Benson, D. F., Cummings, J. L., Russell, G. L., Boone, K. et al. 1997. The temporal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain 120: 1027–40.Google Scholar
Miller, B. L., Seeley, W. W., Mychack, P., Rosen, H. J., Mena, I. and Boone, K.. 2001. Neuroanatomy of the self: evidence from patients with frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 57: 817–21. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11552010.Google Scholar
Wu, T. Q., Miller, Z. A., Adhimoolam, B., Zackey, D. D., Khan, B. K., Ketelle, R. et al. 2013. Verbal creativity in semantic variant primary progressive aphasia. Neurocase, 12 December. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24329034.Google Scholar
Block, N. R., Perry, D. C., Sturm, V. E., Miller, Z. A. and Miller, B. L.. 2013. Religious Dogmatism in Neurodegenerative Disease. Boston: Alzheimer’s Association International Conference.Google Scholar
Kapogiannis, D., Barbey, A. K., Su, M., Krueger, F. and Grafman, J.. 2009. Neuroanatomical variability of religiosity. PLoS ONE 4(9): e7180.Google Scholar
Han, S., Mao, L., Gu, X., Zhu, Y., Ge, J. and Ma, Y.. 2008. Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Social Neuroscience 3: 115. doi:10.1080/17470910701469681.Google Scholar
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M, Bermpohl, F, Dobrowolny, H and Panksepp, J.. 2006. Self-referential processing in our brain – a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage 31: 440–57.Google Scholar
Northoff, G., and Bermpohl, F.. 2004. Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 102–7.Google Scholar
Grèzes, J., Frith, C. D. and Passingham, R. E.. 2004. Inferring false beliefs from the actions of oneself and others: an fMRI study. Neuroimage 21: 744–50. www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1053811903006657.Google Scholar
Azari, N., Nickel, J., Wunderlich, G, Niedeggen, M, Hefter, H, Tellmann, L et al. 2001. Neural correlates of religious experience. European Journal 13: 1649–52. http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01527.x/full.Google Scholar
Newberg, A., Pourdehnad, M., Alavi, A and d’Aquili, E. G.. 2003. Cerebral blood flow during meditative prayer: preliminary findings and methodological issues. Perceptual and Motor Skills 97: 625–30. www.amsciepub.com/doi/abs/10.2466/pms.2003.97.2.625.Google Scholar
Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M, Pourdehnad, M, Santanna, J and d’Aquili, E.. 2001. The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 106: 113–22. www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0925492701000749.Google Scholar
Beauregard, M., and Paquette, V.. 2006. Neural correlates of a mystical experience in Carmelite nuns. Neuroscience Letters 405: 186–90. www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0304394006006392.Google Scholar
Owen, A. D., Hayward, R. D., Koenig, H. G, Steffens, D. C and Payne, M. E.. 2011. Religious factors and hippocampal atrophy in late life. PLoS One 6, no. 3: e17006. www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3068149.Google Scholar
Benson, D. F. 1991. The Geschwind syndrome. Advances in Neurology 55: 411–21.Google Scholar
Dewhurst, K., and Beard, A. W.. 2003. Sudden religious conversions in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior 4: 7887.Google Scholar
Postiglione, A., Milan, G., Pappatà, S, De Falco, C, Lamenza, F, Schiattarella, V, Gallotta, G, Sorrentino, P and Striano, S.. 2008. Fronto-temporal dementia presenting as Geschwind’s syndrome. Neurocase 14: 264–70. doi:10.1080/13554790802269976.Google Scholar
Mazzarella, E., Ramsey, R., Conson, M and Hamilton, A.. 2013. Brain systems for visual perspective taking and action perception. Social Neuroscience 8: 248–67. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23350907.Google Scholar
van Veluw, S. J., and Chance, S. A.. 2014. Differentiating between self and others: an ALE meta-analysis of fMRI studies of self-recognition and theory of mind. Brain Imaging and Behavior 8: 2438. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24535033.Google Scholar
Irish, M., Piguet, O. and Hodges, J. R.. 2011. Self-projection and the default network in frontotemporal dementia. Nature Reviews Neurology 8: 152–61. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22331029.Google Scholar
Call, J., and Tomasello, M.. 2008. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Science 12: 187–92. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/18424224.Google Scholar
Frith, C. 1992. The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Ed. Erlbaum, L.. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Dein, S., and Littlewood, R.. 2011. Religion and psychosis: a common evolutionary trajectory? Transcultural Psychiatry 48: 318–35. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21742955.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×