Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T00:34:01.176Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Linguistic Typology and Hunter-Gatherer Languages

from Part I - Introduction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 February 2020

Tom Güldemann
Affiliation:
Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin
Patrick McConvell
Affiliation:
Australian National University, Canberra
Richard A. Rhodes
Affiliation:
University of California, Berkeley
Get access

Summary

It has long seemed plausible that the languages of hunter-gatherer societies might be systematically different from those of food producers. Compared to food-producing societies, hunter-gatherer societies are usually smaller and less complex, with lower population density. They are often based on kinship as a main organizing factor and usually lack large-scale sociopolitical organization with its concomitant traits such as language standardization.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bickel, Balthasar. (2008). A refined sampling procedure for genealogical control. Language Typology and Universals 61: 221233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bickel, B. (2011). Statistical modeling of language universals. Linguistic Typology 15: 401414.Google Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar. (2013). Distributional biases in language families. In Bickel, B., Grenoble, L. A., Peterson, D. A., and Timberlake, A. (eds.), Language typology and historical contingency: A festschrift to honor Johanna Nichols. Amsterdam: Benjamins, 415444.Google Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar, Nichols, Johanna, Zakharko, Taras, Witzlack-Makarevich, Alena, Hildebrandt, Kristine, Rießler, Michael, Bierkandt, Lennart, Zúñiga, Fernando, and Lowe, John B.. (2017). The AUTOTYP typological databases. Version 0.1.0. https://github.com/autotyp/autotyp-data/tree/0.1.0Google Scholar
Bickel, Balthasar, and Nichols, Johanna. (2006). Oceania, the Pacific Rim, and the theory of linguistic areas. In Z. Antić, C.Chang, C. Sandy, and M. Toosarvandani (eds.), Proceedings of the 32nd Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Special Session on the Languages and Linguistics of Oceania. 315.Google Scholar
Donohue, Mark, and Nichols, Johanna. (2011). Does phoneme inventory size correlate with population size? Linguistic Typology 15(2): 161170.Google Scholar
Dryer, Matthew S. (1989). Large linguistic areas and language sampling. Studies in Language 13: 257292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin, Dryer, Matthew S., Gil, David, and Comrie, Bernard (eds.) (2005). The world atlas of language structures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Moran, Steven, McCloy, Daniel, and Wright, Richard. (2012). Revisiting population size vs. phoneme inventory size. Language 88(4): 877893.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. (1992). Linguistic diversity in space and time. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. (2009). Linguistic complexity: A comprehensive definition and survey. In Sampson, Geoffrey, Gil, David, and Trudgill, Peter (eds.), Language complexity as an evolving variable. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 110125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. (2011). Monogenesis or polygenesis: A single ancestral language for all humanity? In Tallerman, Maggie and Gibson, Kathleen R. (eds.), The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 558572.Google Scholar
Nichols, Johanna. (2015). Types of spread zones: Open and closed, horizontal and vertical. In De Busser, Rik and La Polla, Randy (eds.), Language structure and environment: Social, cultural, and natural factors. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
R Development Core Team. (2011). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing, www.r-project.org.Google Scholar
Trudgill, Peter. (2011). Sociolinguistic typology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×