Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T23:37:35.420Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Resumptive Pronouns in English

from Part II - Experimental Studies of Specific Phenomena

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 December 2021

Grant Goodall
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
Get access

Summary

In many introspective and corpus studies, inserting a resumptive pronoun in place of a gap in island-violating wh-dependency structures in English is said to amnesty, ameliorate, or repair the island violation, improving the acceptability of otherwise unacceptable structures. Most experimental studies on the acceptability of such resumptive structures, however, report that native speakers of English do not judge island-violating dependency structures with resumptive pronouns to be more acceptable than the ones with gaps. But studies testing the comprehensibility and processing of resumption report that resumptive pronouns increase the comprehensibility of island-violating structures and facilitate processing of long dependencies. These results taken together suggest that although resumptive pronouns in islands do not have an ameliorating effect on grammaticality, they may confer a processing benefit. A question, however, remains as to whether the reported enhanced comprehensibility and ease in processing actually increase the accuracy in interpreting the resumptive pronouns.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ackerman, L., Frazier, M., & Yoshida, M. (2018). Resumptive pronouns can ameliorate illicit island extractions. Linguistic Inquiry, 49, 847859.Google Scholar
Alexopoulou, T. & Keller, F. (2007). Locality, cyclicity, and resumption: At the interface between the grammar and the human sentence processor. Language, 83, 110160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ariel, M. (1999). Cognitive universals and linguistic conventions: The case of resumptive pronouns. Studies in Language, 23, 217269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asudeh, A. (2011). Local grammaticality in syntactic production. In Bender, E. M. & Arnold, J. E., eds., Language from a Cognitive Perspective. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications, pp. 5179.Google Scholar
Asudeh, A. (2012). The Logic of Pronominal Resumption. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Asudeh, A. (2013). Directionality and production of ungrammatical sentences. Studies in Linguistics, 6, 83106.Google Scholar
Beltrama, A. & Xiang, M. (2016). Unacceptable but comprehensible: the facilitation effect of resumptive pronouns. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 1(1), 29. DOI:10.5334/gjgl.24Google Scholar
Cann, R., Kaplan, T., & Kempson, R. (2005). Data at the grammar-pragmatics interface: the case of resumptive pronouns in English. Lingua, 115, 15511577.Google Scholar
Clemens, L. E., Morgan, A., Polinsky, M., & Xiang, M. (2012). Listening to resumptives: An auditory experiment. A poster presented at CUNY 2012 Conference on Human Sentence Processing, March 14–16, 2012, CUNY.Google Scholar
Dickey, M. W. (1996). Constraints on the sentence processor and the distribution of resumptive pronouns. In Dickey, M. W. & Tunstall, S., eds., Linguistics in the Laboratory. Amherst, MA: GLSA, pp. 157192.Google Scholar
Erteschik-Shir, N. (1992). Resumptive pronouns in islands. In Goodluck, H. & Rochemont, M., eds., Island Constraints: Theory, Acquisition and Processing (Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics 15). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp. 89109.Google Scholar
Fadlon, J., Morgan, A. M., Meltzer-Asscher, A., & Ferreira, V. S. (2019). It depends: Optionality in the production of filler-gap dependencies. Journal of Memory and Language, 106, 4076.Google Scholar
Ferreira, F. & Swets, B. (2005). The production and comprehension of resumptive pronouns in relative clause “island” contexts. In Cutler, A., ed., Twenty-First Century Psycholinguistics: Four Cornerstones. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 263278.Google Scholar
Goodall, G. (2017). Referentiality and resumption in wh-dependencies. In Ostrove, J., Kramer, R., & Sabbagh, J., eds., Asking the Right Questions: Essays in Honor of Sandra Chung. eScholarship, University of California, pp. 6580.Google Scholar
Han, C. Elouazizi, N., Galeano, C., Görgülü, E., Hedberg, N., Hinnell, J., Jeffrey, M., Kim, K., & Kirby, S. (2012). Processing strategies and resumptive pronouns in English. In Arnett, N. & Bennett, R., eds., Proceedings of the 30th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 153161.Google Scholar
Heestand, D., Xiang, M. & Polinsky, M. (2011). Resumption still does not rescue islands. Linguistic Inquiry, 42, 138152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmeister, P. & Norcliffe, E. (2013). Does resumption facilitate sentence comprehension? In Hofmeister, P. & Norcliffe, E., eds., The Core and the Periphery: Data-Driven Perspectives on Syntax Inspired by Ivan A. Sag. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.Google Scholar
Kayne, R. (1981). “ECP” Extensions. Linguistic Inquiry, 12, 93134.Google Scholar
Keffala, B. (2013). Resumption and gaps in English relative clauses: Relative acceptability creates an illusion of “saving.” In C. Chen, Cathcart, G. Kang, Finley, C. S. Sandy, , & E. Stickles, , eds., Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley Linguistics Society, pp. 140154.Google Scholar
Keffala, B. & Goodall, G. (2011). Do resumptive pronouns ever rescue illicit gaps in English? A poster presented at CUNY 2011 Conference on Human Sentence Processing, March 24–26, 2011, Stanford University.Google Scholar
Kroch, A. (1981). On the role of resumptive pronouns in amnestying island constraint violations. In Hendrick, R. A., Masek, C. S., & Miller, M. F., eds., Papers from the 17th Regional Meeting, Chicago Linguistic Society. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society, pp. 125135.Google Scholar
McCloskey, J. (1990). Resumptive pronouns, A’-binding and levels of representation in Irish. In Hendrick, R., ed., Syntax of the Modern Celtic Languages. New York: Academic Press, pp. 199248.Google Scholar
McCloskey, J. (2017). New thoughts on old questions: Resumption in Irish. In Ostrove, J., Kramer, R., & Sabbagh, J., eds., Asking the Right Questions: Essays in Honor of Sandra Chung. eScholarship, University of California, pp. 81102.Google Scholar
McDaniel, D. & Cowart, W. (1999). Experimental evidence for a minimalist account of English resumptive pronouns. Cognition, 70, B15B24.Google Scholar
Meltzer-Asscher, A., Fadlon, J., Goldstein, K., & Holan, A. (2015). Direct object resumption in Hebrew: How modality of presentation and relative clause position affect acceptability. Lingua, 166, 6579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, A. M., von der Malsburg, T., Ferreira, V. S., & Wittenberg, E. (2018). This is the structure that we wonder why anyone produces it: Resumptive pronouns in English hinder sentence comprehension. Presented at the 2018 CUNY Conference on Human Sentence Processing, MIT, Boston.Google Scholar
Morgan, A. M. & Wagers, M. W. (2018). English resumptive pronouns are more common where gaps are less acceptable. Linguistic Inquiry, 49, 861876.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M., Clemens, L. E., Morgan, A. M., Xiang, M., & Heestand, D. (2013). Resumption in English. In Sprouse, J., ed., Experimental Syntax and Island Effects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 341360.Google Scholar
Prince, E. (1990). Syntax and discourse: A look at resumptive pronouns. In Hall, K., Koenig, J.-P., Meacham, M., Reinman, S., & Sutton, L. A., eds., Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: Parasession on the Legacy of Grice. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley Linguistics Society, pp. 482497.Google Scholar
Ross, J. R. (1967). Constraints on variables in syntax. Doctoral dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Sells, P. (1984). Syntax and semantics of resumptive pronouns. Doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.Google Scholar
Shlonsky, U. (1992). Resumptive pronouns as a last resort. Linguistic Inquiry, 23, 443468.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×