Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T13:24:23.420Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 February 2020

Carolyn Dicey Jennings
Affiliation:
University of California, Merced
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
The Attending Mind , pp. 218 - 246
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aegerter, C., Günther, R., and Wijngaarden, R. (2003). Avalanche dynamics, surface roughening, and self-organized criticality: Experiments on a three-dimensional pile of rice. Physical Review E, 67(5):051306.Google Scholar
Aguilar, J. H. and Buckareff, A. A. (2009). Agency, consciousness, and executive control. Philosophia, 37(1):2130.Google Scholar
Allport, D. A. (1987). Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action. In Heuer, H. and Sanders, A. F., editors, Perspectives on perception and action, pages 395419. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1955). Becoming: Basic considerations for a psychology of personality, volume 20. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT and London.Google Scholar
Alvarez, G. and Oliva, A. (2009). Spatial ensemble statistics are efficient codes that can be represented with reduced attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(18):7345–50.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). American Psychiatric Publishing, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Andersen, R. A. (1997). Multimodal integration for the representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B: Biological Sciences, 352(1360):1421–8.Google Scholar
Anderson, P. W. (1972). More is different. Science, 177(4047):393–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Annas, J. (2011). Intelligent virtue. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Arnsten, A. (2006). Fundamentals of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Circuits and pathways. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 67:712.Google Scholar
Ashby, F. G., Turner, B. O., and Horvitz, J. C. (2010). Cortical and basal ganglia contributions to habit learning and automaticity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(5):208–15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Asherson, P., Ramos-Quiroga, J. A., and Young, S. (2018). Adult ADHD: Clinical presentation and assessment. In Banaschewski, T., Coghill, D., and Zuddas, A., editors, Oxford textbook of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, pages 178–89. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Asplund, C. L., Todd, J. J., Snyder, A. P., and Marois, R. (2010). A central role for the lateral prefrontal cortex in goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention. Nature Neuroscience, 13(4):507–12.Google Scholar
Astell, M. (2017). A serious proposal to the ladies, for the advancement of their true and greatest interest. Gutenberg Books. Retrieved from www.gutenberg.org/ebooks/54984.Google Scholar
Atmanspacher, H. (2007). Contextual emergence from physics to cognitive neuroscience. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14(1–2):1836.Google Scholar
Attend” (2019). Oxford English dictionary. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Augustine, S. (1888). The city of God (M. Dods, Trans.). T & T Clark, Evanston, IL.Google Scholar
Augustine, S. (2003). City of God (B. Henry, Trans.). Penguin Classics, London.Google Scholar
Avela, J. and Komi, P. V. (1998). Reduced stretch reflex sensitivity and muscle stiffness after long-lasting stretch-shortening cycle exercise in humans. European Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 78(5):403–10.Google ScholarPubMed
Awh, E. and Jonides, J. (2001). Overlapping mechanisms of attention and spatial working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(3):119–26.Google Scholar
Baars, B. (1983). Conscious contents provide the nervous system with coherent, global information. In Davidson, R. J., Schwartz, G. E., and Shapiro, D. H., editors, Consciousness & self-regulation, pages 4179. Plenum Publishing Corporation, New York.Google Scholar
Baars, B. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Baars, B. J. (1997). Some essential differences between consciousness and attention, perception, and working memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 6:363–71.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(11):417–23.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (2010). Working memory. Current Biology, 20(4):R136–40.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. and Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G. H., editor, The psychology of learning and motivation, Vol. 8, pages 4789. Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. and Hitch, G. J. (2000). Development of working memory: Should the Pascual-Leone and the Baddeley and Hitch models be merged? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 77(2):128–37.Google Scholar
Baker, L. R. (2009). Nonreductive materialism. In McLaughlin, B., Beckermann, A., and Walter, S., editors, The Oxford handbook of philosophy of mind, pages 109–27. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Baluch, F. and Itti, L. (2011). Mechanisms of top-down attention. Trends in Neurosciences, 34(4):210–24.Google Scholar
Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1):6594.Google Scholar
Baxter, D. L. (1997). Abstraction, inseparability, and identity. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 57(2):307–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayne, T. J. and Chalmers, D. J. (2003). What is the unity of consciousness? In Cleeremans, A., editor, The unity of consciousness, pages 2358. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Beck, D. M. and Kastner, S. (2009). Top-down and bottom-up mechanisms in biasing competition in the human brain. Vision Research, 49(10):1154–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedau, M. (2002). Downward causation and the autonomy of weak emergence. Principia, 6(1):550.Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L. and Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4):701–25.Google ScholarPubMed
Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., and Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8(1):616.Google Scholar
Beilock, S. L. and Gray, R. (2012). From attentional control to attentional spillover: A skill-level investigation of attention, movement, and performance outcomes. Human Movement Science, 31(6):1473–99.Google Scholar
Bellgrove, M. A., Johnson, K. A., Barry, E., Mulligan, A., Hawi, Z., Gill, M., Robertson, I., and Chambers, C. D. (2009). Dopaminergic haplotype as a predictor of spatial inattention in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(10):1135–42.Google Scholar
Berger, L. (2017). Attention as the way to being. Retrieved from www.academia.edu/31329912/Attention_as_the_Way_to_Being.Google Scholar
Bergson, H. (1920). Mind-energy: Lectures and essays (H. W. Carr, Trans.). McMillan & Company, Westport, CT.Google Scholar
Bergson, H. (2007). Matter and memory (N. M. Paul and W. S. Palmer, Trans.). Cosimo Classics, New York.Google Scholar
Berkeley, G. (1881). A treatise concerning the principles of human knowledge. JB Lippincott & Co., Philadelphia. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.78.4043.Google Scholar
Berkow, I. (1989). Sports of the times; Sugar Ray danced in a deadly game. New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/1989/04/15/sports/sports-of-the-times-sugar-ray-danced-in-a-deadly-game.html.Google Scholar
Bermúdez, J. P. (2017). Do we reflect while performing skillful actions? Automaticity, control, and the perils of distraction. Philosophical Psychology, 30(7): 896924.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berridge, K. C. and Kringelbach, M. L. (2008). Affective neuroscience of pleasure: Reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology, 199(3):457–80.Google Scholar
Bhatla, N., Droste, R., Sando, S. R., Huang, A., and Horvitz, H. R. (2015). Distinct neural circuits control rhythm inhibition and spitting by the myogenic pharynx of C. elegans. Current Biology, 25(16):2075–89.Google Scholar
Bhatla, N. and Horvitz, H. R. (2015). Light and hydrogen peroxide inhibit C. elegans feeding through gustatory receptor orthologs and pharyngeal neurons. Neuron, 85(4):804–18.Google Scholar
Bichot, N. and Desimone, R. (2006). Finding a face in the crowd: Parallel and serial neural mechanisms of visual selection. Progress in Brain Research, 155:147–56.Google Scholar
Binks, D. and van de Water, W. (1997). Nonlinear pattern formation of Faraday waves. Physical Review Letters, 78(21):4043–46.Google Scholar
Bishop, R. C. and Atmanspacher, H. (2006). Contextual emergence in the description of properties. Foundations of Physics, 36(12):1753–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Block, N. (1990). Consciousness and Accessibility. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(4):596–78.Google Scholar
Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18:227–47.Google Scholar
Block, N. (1998). How to find the neural correlate of consciousness. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplements, 43:2334.Google Scholar
Block, N. (2005). Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2):4652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, N. ( 2008a ). Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5–6):481–99.Google Scholar
Block, N. ( 2008b ). Consciousness and cognitive access. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 108(1): 289317.Google Scholar
Block, N. (2014). Rich conscious perception outside focal attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(9):445–7.Google Scholar
Boes, A. D., Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Anderson, S. W., Richman, L., and Nopoulos, P. (2008). Right ventromedial prefrontal cortex: A neuroanatomical correlate of impulse control in boys. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuro-science, 4(1):19.Google ScholarPubMed
Borji, A. and Itti, L. (2014). Defending Yarbus: Eye movements reveal observers’ task. Journal of Vision, 14(3), art. 29.Google Scholar
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., and Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12):539–46.Google Scholar
Box, S. and Martin, E. (2006). Breakthrough research on real-world driver behavior released. National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Retrieved from www.nhtsa.gov/Driving+Safety/Distracted+Driving+at+Distraction.gov/Breakthrough+Research+on+Real-World+Driver+Behavior+Released.Google Scholar
Bradley, F. H. (1886). Is there any special activity of attention? Mind, 11(43):305–23.Google Scholar
Braver, T. S. (2007). Working memory. In Smith, E. E. and Kosslyn, S. M., editors, Cognition: Mind and brain, pages 239–78. Prentice Hall, New York.Google Scholar
Brisson, B., Leblanc, E., and Jolicoeur, P. (2009). Contingent capture of visual-spatial attention depends on capacity-limited central mechanisms: Evidence from human electrophysiology and the psychological refractory period. Biological Psychology, 80(2):218–25.Google Scholar
Broad, J. (2015). The philosophy of Mary Astell: An early modern theory of virtue. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and communication. Pergamon Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Brodersen, K. H., Wiech, K., Lomakina, E. I., Lin, C., Buhmann, J. M., Bingel, U., Ploner, M., Stephan, K. E., and Tracey, I. (2012). Decoding the perception of pain from fMRI using multivariate pattern analysis. Neuroimage, 63(3):1162–70.Google Scholar
Brown, D. (2007). Augustine and Descartes on the function of attention in perceptual awareness. In Heinämaa, S., Lähteenmäki, V., and Remes, P., editors, Consciousness: From perception to reflection in the history of philosophy, pages 153–75. Springer, Dordrecht.Google Scholar
Brown, S. R. (2013). Emergence in the central nervous system. Cognitive Neurodynamics, 7(3):173–95.Google Scholar
Brown, T. E. (2009). ADD/ADHD and impaired executive function in clinical practice. Current Attention Disorders Reports, 1(1):3741.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brownstein, M. (2014). Rationalizing flow: Agency in skilled unreflective action. Philosophical Studies, 168(2):545–68.Google Scholar
Bruya, B. (2010). Effortless attention: A new perspective in the cognitive science of attention and action. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Bullmore, E. and Sporns, O. (2012). The economy of brain network organization. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(5):336–49.Google Scholar
Buras, T. (2009). The function of sensations in Reid. Journal of the History of Philosophy, 47(3):329–53.Google Scholar
Burge, T. (2010). Origins of objectivity. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Buschman, T. J. and Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science, 315(5820):1860–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Busse, L., Katzner, S., Tillmann, C., and Treue, S. (2008). Effects of attention on perceptual direction tuning curves in the human visual system. Journal of Vision, 8(9):113.Google Scholar
Buzsáki, G. and Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science, 304(5679):1926–9.Google Scholar
Buzsáki, G. and Wang, X. (2012). Mechanisms of gamma oscillations. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35:203–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caldwell, B. (2010). Beyond positivism. Routledge, London.Google Scholar
Calipari, E. S., Ferris, M. J., Salahpour, A., Caron, M. G., and Jones, S. R. (2013). Methylphenidate amplifies the potency and reinforcing effects of amphetamines by increasing dopamine transporter expression. Nature Communications, 4:2720.Google Scholar
Campbell, J. (2002). Reference and consciousness. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Campbell, J. (2004). Reference as attention. Philosophical Studies, 120(1):265–76.Google Scholar
Campbell, J. (2012). Inductions about attention and consciousness: Comments on Carolyn Suchy-Dicey, “Inductive scepticism and the methodological argument.” Consciousness and Cognition, 21(2):610–12.Google Scholar
Cao, B., Li, W., Li, F., and Li, H. (2016). Dissociable roles of medial and lateral PFC in rule learning. Brain and Behavior, 6(11):110.Google Scholar
Cao, R. (2012). A teleosemantic approach to information in the brain. Biology & Philosophy, 27(1):4971.Google Scholar
Carrasco, M. (2011). Visual attention: The past 25 years. Vision Research, 51(13):14841525.Google Scholar
Carrasco, M., Ling, S., and Read, S. (2004). Attention alters appearance. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3):308–13.Google Scholar
Carter, C. S. and Krug, M. K. (2012). Dynamic cognitive control and frontal-cingulate interactions. In Posner, M. I., editor, Cognitive neuroscience of attention, pages 89–98. Guilford Press, New York, second edition.Google Scholar
Cartwright-Finch, U. and Lavie, N. (2007). The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition, 102(3):321–40.Google Scholar
Castellanos, F. X. and Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(8):617–28.Google Scholar
Cavagna, A., Cimarelli, A., Giardina, I., Parisi, G., Santagati, R., Stefanini, F., and Viale, M. (2010). Scale-free correlations in starling flocks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(26):11865–70.Google Scholar
Cerbone, D. R. (2012). Phenomenological method: Reflection, introspection, and skepticism. In Zahavi, D., editor, The Oxford handbook of contemporary phenomenology. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Chadderton, P., Agapiou, J. P., McAlpine, D., and Margrie, T. W. (2009). The synaptic representation of sound source location in auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(45):14127–35.Google Scholar
Chaffin, R. and Imreh, G. (2002). Practicing perfection: Piano performance as expert memory. Psychological Science, 13(4):342–9.Google Scholar
Chakrabarty, M., Badgio, D., Ptacek, J., Biswas, A., Ghosal, M., and Chatterjee, G. (2017). Hemispheric asymmetry in attention and its impact on our consciousness: A review with reference to altered consciousness in right hemisphere damaged subjects. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 24(7–8):5178.Google Scholar
Chalmers, D. J. (1996). The conscious mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Chalmers, D. J. (2008). Foreword to Andy Clark’s “Surprising the mind.” In Clark, A., editor, Surprising the mind: Embodiment, action and cognitive extension. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Chambers, C. D., Payne, J. M., Stokes, M. G., and Mattingley, J. B. (2004). Fast and slow parietal pathways mediate spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 7(3):217–18.Google Scholar
Chaucer, G. (1868). Chaucer’s translation of Boethius’s “De consolatione philosophiae. Oxford University Press, London.Google Scholar
Chechlacz, M. and Humphreys, G. W. (2014). The enigma of Balint’s syndrome: Neural substrates and cognitive deficits. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8:13. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25(5):975–9.Google Scholar
Chialvo, D. R. (2010). Emergent complex neural dynamics. Nature Physics, 6(10):744–50.Google Scholar
Chiang, C., Shivacharan, R. S., Wei, X., Gonzalez-Reyes, L. E., and Durand, D. M. (2019). Slow periodic activity in the longitudinal hippocampal slice can self-propagate non-synaptically by a mechanism consistent with ephaptic coupling. The Journal of Physiology, 597(1):249–69.Google Scholar
Christensen, W., Sutton, J., and McIlwain, D. J. (2016). Cognition in skilled action: Meshed control and the varieties of skill experience. Mind & Language, 31(1):3766.Google Scholar
Chua, K. P. and Chun, M. M. (2003). Implicit scene learning is viewpoint dependent. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(1):7280.Google Scholar
Coates, P. (2004). Wilfrid Sellars, perceptual consciousness and theories of attention. Essays in Philosophy, 5(1):125.Google Scholar
Collins, P. and White, T. (2002). Forensic implications of attention deficit hyper-activity disorder (ADHD) in adulthood. The Journal of Forensic Psychiatry, 13(2):263–84.Google Scholar
Connor, C. E., Egeth, H. E., and Yantis, S. (2004). Visual attention: Bottom-up versus top-down. Current Biology, 14(19):R850–2.Google Scholar
Corbetta, M., Kincade, M. J., Lewis, C., Snyder, A. Z., and Sapir, A. (2005). Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11):1603–10.Google Scholar
Courtney, S. M. (2004). Attention and cognitive control as emergent properties of information representation in working memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 4(4):501–16.Google Scholar
Coutureau, E. and Killcross, S. (2003). Inactivation of the infralimbic prefrontal cortex reinstates goal-directed responding in overtrained rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 146(1–2):167–74.Google Scholar
Crescentini, C., Seyed-Allaei, S., De Pisapia, N., Jovicich, J., Amati, D., and Shallice, T. (2011). Mechanisms of rule acquisition and rule following in inductive reasoning. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(21):7763–74.Google Scholar
Crick, F. and Koch, C. (1990). Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. In Seminars in the neurosciences, volume 2, pages 263–75, Philadelphia, Saunders Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
Crone, E. A., Donohue, S. E., Honomichl, R., Wendelken, C., and Bunge, S. A. (2006). Brain regions mediating flexible rule use during development. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(43):11239–47.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. and Csikszentmihalyi, I. (1992). Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
Cubillo, A., Halari, R., Giampietro, V., Taylor, E., and Rubia, K. (2011). Fronto-striatal underactivation during interference inhibition and attention allocation in grown up children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and persistent symptoms. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 193(1):1727.Google Scholar
da Silva Simões, A., Colombini, E. L., and Ribeiro, C. H. C. (2017). Conaim: A conscious attention-based integrated model for human-like robots. IEEE Systems Journal, 11(3):12961307.Google Scholar
Dahlstrom, D. O. (2013). The Heidegger dictionary. A&C Black, London.Google Scholar
Datta, R. and DeYoe, E. A. (2009). I know where you are secretly attending! The topography of human visual attention revealed with fMRI. Vision Research, 49(10):1037–44.Google Scholar
Davidson, D. (1971). Agency. In Binkley, R., Bronaugh, R., and Marras, A., editors, Agent, action, and reason, pages 325. University of Toronto Press, Toronto.Google Scholar
Davoodi, R., Moradi, M. H., and Yoonessi, A. (2015). Dissociation between attention and consciousness during a novel task: An ERP study. Neurophysiology, 47(2):144–54.Google Scholar
Daw, N. D., Niv, Y., and Dayan, P. (2005). Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nature Neuroscience, 8(12):1704–11.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F. (2010). Consciousness, attention and commonsense. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 17(9–10):189201.Google Scholar
De Brigard, F. and Prinz, J. (2010). Attention and consciousness. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(1):51–9.Google Scholar
de Gelder, B., Tamietto, M., van Boxtel, G., Goebel, R., Sahraie, A., van den Stock, J., Stienen, B., Weiskrantz, L., and Pegna, A. (2008). Intact navigation skills after bilateral loss of striate cortex. Current Biology, 18(24):1128–9.Google Scholar
Debner, J. A. and Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Unconscious perception: Attention, awareness, and control. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20:304–17.Google Scholar
Debus, D. (2015). Losing oneself (in a good way): On the value of full attention. European Journal of Philosophy, 23(4):1174–91.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., and Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5):204–11.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. and Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79:137.Google Scholar
del Campo, N., Chamberlain, S. R., Sahakian, B. J., and Robbins, T. W. (2011). The roles of dopamine and noradrenaline in the pathophysiology and treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 69(12):e145–57.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1982). Styles of mental representation. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, 83:213–26.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1993). Consciousness explained. Penguin Books, London.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. and Kinsbourne, M. (1992). Time and the observer: The where and when of consciousness in the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15(2):183201.Google Scholar
Depraz, N. (2004). Where is the phenomenology of attention that Husserl intended to perform? A transcendental pragmatic-oriented description of attention. Continental Philosophy Review, 37(1):520.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1901). The method, meditations and philosophy of Descartes. M. Walter Dunne, Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Desimone, R. and Duncan, J. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18(1):193222.Google Scholar
Deutsch, J. A. and Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70(1):8090.Google Scholar
deVries, W. A. (1988). Hegel’s theory of mental activity. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.Google Scholar
Dickie, I. (2011). Visual attention fixes demonstrative reference by eliminating referential luck. In Mole, C., Smithies, D., and Wu, W., editors, Attention: Philosophical and psychological essays, pages 292322. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Dreyfus, S. E. (2004). The five-stage model of adult skill acquisition. Bulletin of Science, Technology & Society, 24(3):177–81.Google Scholar
Du Bois, W. E. B. (2008). The souls of black folk. Gutenberg Books. Retrieved from www.gutenberg.org/ebooks/408.Google Scholar
Duke, R. A., Cash, C. D., and Allen, S. E. (2011). Focus of attention affects performance of motor skills in music. Journal of Research in Music Education, 59(1):4455.Google Scholar
Duhamel, J. R., Colby, C. L., and Goldberg, M. (1992). The updating of the representation of visual space in parietal cortex by intended eye movements. Science, 255(5040):90–2.Google Scholar
Eimer, M., and Driver, J. (2001). Crossmodal links in endogenous and exogenous spatial attention: Evidence from event-related brain potential studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 25(6): 497511.Google Scholar
Eme, R. (2014). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A mitigation of criminal responsibility? Journal of Forensic Psychology Practice, 14(3):221–36.Google Scholar
Eme, R. (2016). The neuroscience of ADHD, the paradigmatic disorder of self-control. Journal of Law and the Biosciences, 3(2):350–4.Google Scholar
Engel, A., Fries, P., and Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: Oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2:704–16.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. and Ward, P. (2007). Capturing the naturally occurring superior performance of experts in the laboratory: Toward a science of expert and exceptional performance. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6):346–50.Google Scholar
Euston, D. R., Gruber, A. J., and McNaughton, B. L. (2012). The role of medial prefrontal cortex in memory and decision making. Neuron, 76(6):1057–70.Google Scholar
Evans, K. and Treisman, A. (2005). Perception of objects in natural scenes: Is it really attention free? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(6):1476–92.Google Scholar
Fazekas, P. and Nanay, B. (2018). Attention is amplification, not selection. The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, pages 170. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000333.Google Scholar
Fei-Fei, L., Iyer, A., Koch, C., and Perona, P. (2007). What do we perceive in a glance of a real-world scene? Journal of Vision, 7(1):10, 129.Google Scholar
Flanagan, O. J. (1992). Consciousness reconsidered. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Ford, J. and Smith, D. W. (2006). Consciousness, self, and attention. In Kriegel, U. and Williford, K., editors, Self-representational approaches to consciousness, pages 353–77. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Frankfurt, H. G. (1969). Alternate possibilities and moral responsibility. The Journal of Philosophy, 66(23):829–39.Google Scholar
Fridland, E. (2014). They’ve lost control: Reflections on skill. Synthese, 191(12):2729–50.Google Scholar
Fröhlich, F. and McCormick, D. A. (2010). Endogenous electric fields may guide neocortical network activity. Neuron, 67(1):129–43.Google Scholar
Fulton, A. (2016). To find food in the wild, you must know how to look. National Geographic. Retrieved from http://theplate.nationalgeographic.com/2016/03/02/to-find-food-in-the-wild-you-must-know-how-to-look/.Google Scholar
Ganeri, J. (2011). Emergentisms, ancient and modern. Mind, 120(479):671703.Google Scholar
Ganeri, J. (2017). Attention, not self. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Ganeri, J. (2018). Attention to greatness: Buddhaghosa. In Hetherington, S., editor, What makes a philosopher great, pages 67–85. Routledge, New York.Google Scholar
Garrett, D. (2009). Descartes, Spinoza, and Locke on extended thinking beings. In Miller, J., editor, Topics in early modern philosophy of mind, pages 85104. Springer, Kingston, Ontario.Google Scholar
Gasché, R. (2008). On seeing away: Attention and abstraction in Kant. CR: The New Centennial Review, 8(3):128.Google Scholar
Gazzaley, A. and Nobre, A. C. (2012). Top-down modulation: Bridging selective attention and working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2):129–35.Google Scholar
Gettier, E. L. (1963). Is justified true belief knowledge? Analysis, 23(6):121–3.Google Scholar
Ghosh, S., Larson, S. D., Hefzi, H., Marnoy, Z., Cutforth, T., Dokka, K., and Baldwin, K. K. (2011). Sensory maps in the olfactory cortex defined by long-range viral tracing of single neurons. Nature, 472(7342):217–20.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Houghton Mifflin, Oxford.Google Scholar
Gibson, R. M., Chennu, S., Fernández-Espejo, D., Naci, L., Owen, A. M., and Cruse, D. (2016). Somatosensory attention identifies both overt and covert awareness in disorders of consciousness. Annals of Neurology, 80(3):412–23.Google Scholar
Gillett, C. (2016). Reduction and emergence in science and philosophy. Cambridge University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Godwin, C. A., Gazzaley, A., and Morsella, E. (2013). Homing in on the brain mechanisms linked to consciousness: The buffer of the perception-and-action interface. In Pereira, Jr., Lehmann, A. D., editors, The unity of mind, brain and world: Current perspectives on a science of consciousness, pages 4376. Cambridge University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Goldberg, I., Harel, M., and Malach, R. (2006). When the brain loses its self: Prefrontal inactivation during sensorimotor processing. Neuron, 50(2): 329–39.Google Scholar
Goldberg, S. (2007). Internalism and externalism in semantics and epistemology. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Goldreich, D. (2007). A Bayesian perceptual model replicates the cutaneous rabbit and other tactile spatiotemporal illusions. PLoS One, 2(3):e333. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000333.Google Scholar
Goodale, M. and Milner, D. (2004). Sight unseen. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Greene, M. R., Liu, T., and Wolfe, J. M. (2012). Reconsidering Yarbus: A failure to predict observers’ task from eye movement patterns. Vision Research, 62:18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gurwitsch, A. (1964). Field of consciousness. Duquesne University Press, Pittsburgh, PA.Google Scholar
Gurwitsch, A. (1985). Marginal consciousness. Ohio University Press, Athens.Google Scholar
Haggard, P. and Johnson, H. (2003). Experiences of voluntary action. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(9–10):7284.Google Scholar
Haladjian, H. H. and Montemayor, C. (2016). Artificial consciousness and the consciousness-attention dissociation. Consciousness and Cognition, 45:210–25.Google Scholar
Han, K., Guo, C., Chen, C. H., Witter, L., Osorno, T., and Regehr, W. G. (2018). Ephaptic coupling promotes synchronous firing of cerebellar purkinje cells. Neuron, 100(3):564–78.Google Scholar
Handford, M. (2012). Where’s Waldo now? Candlewick Press, Candlewick, MA.Google Scholar
Handwerker, D. A. and Bandettini, P. A. (2011). Hemodynamic signals not predicted? Not so: A comment on Sirotin and Das (2009). Neuroimage, 55(4):1409–12.Google Scholar
Hannon, E. M. and Richards, A. (2010). Is inattentional blindness related to individual differences in visual working memory capacity or executive control functioning? Perception, 39(3):309–19.Google Scholar
Harvey, R. C., Sen, S., Deaciuc, A., Dwoskin, L. P., and Kantak, K. M. (2011). Methylphenidate treatment in adolescent rats with an attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder phenotype: Cocaine addiction vulnerability and dopamine transporter function. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(4):837–47.Google Scholar
Hasson, U., Nir, Y., Levy, I., Fuhrmann, G., and Malach, R. (2004). Inter-subject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science, 303(5664):1634–40.Google Scholar
He, B. J., Zempel, J. M., Snyder, A. Z., and Raichle, M. E. (2010). The temporal structures and functional significance of scale-free brain activity. Neuron, 66(3):353–69.Google Scholar
Head, H. and Holmes, G. (1911). Sensory disturbances from cerebral lesions. Brain, 34(2–3):102254.Google Scholar
Hegel, G. W. F. (1971). Philosophy of mind (W. Wallace, Trans.). Clarendon Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Hegel, G. W. F. (2007). Hegel: Philosophy of mind (M. Inwood, Trans.). Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Heidegger, M. (1978). Being and time. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford.Google Scholar
Heilbronner, S. R. and Hayden, B. Y. (2016). Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A bottom-up view. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 39:149–70.Google Scholar
Henry, A. and Bayne, T. (2013). Review of Attention Is Cognitive Unison: An Essay in Philosophical Psychology, by Christopher Mole. Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 91(1):199202.Google Scholar
Herrmann, K., Montaser-Kouhsari, L., Carrasco, M., and Heeger, D. (2010). When size matters: Attention affects performance by contrast or response gain. Nature Neuroscience, 13(12):1554–9.Google Scholar
Heuer, U. (2014). Intentions and the reasons for which we act. In Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society, volume 114, pages 291315, Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Heylighen, F. (2008). Complexity and self-organization. In Bates, M. J. and Maack, M. N., editors, Encyclopedia of library and information sciences, volume 3, pages 1215–24. Taylor & Francis, London.Google Scholar
Hirst, W., Spelke, E., Reaves, C., Caharack, G., and Neisser, U. (1980). Dividing attention without alternation or automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 109(1):98117.Google Scholar
Hobson, J. (2009). REM sleep and dreaming: Towards a theory of protoconsciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(11):803–13.Google Scholar
Hochstein, S. and Ahissar, M. (2002). View from the top: Hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron, 36(5):791804.Google Scholar
Hopp, W. (2008). Husserl on sensation, perception, and interpretation. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 38(2):219–45.Google Scholar
Horton, J. C. and Adams, D. L. (2005). The cortical column: A structure without a function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, 360(1456):837–62.Google Scholar
Howell, R. J. (2009). The ontology of subjective physicalism. Noûs, 43(2):315–45.Google Scholar
Humphreys, P. (2016). Emergence. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Humphries, C., Liebenthal, E., and Binder, J. R. (2010). Tonotopic organization of human auditory cortex. Neuroimage, 50(3):1202–11.Google Scholar
Hunt, D. P. (2000). Moral responsibility and unavoidable action. Philosophical Studies, 97(2):195227.Google Scholar
Hurley, S. L. (2002). Consciousness in action. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1975). Experience and judgment (J. S. Churchill and K. Ameriks, Trans.). Northwestern University Press, Evanston, IL.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (2001). Logical investigations (J. N. Findlay, Trans.). Northwestern University Press, London.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (2012). Analyses concerning passive and active synthesis: Lectures on transcendental logic, volume 9. Springer Science & Business Media, Carbondale, IL.Google Scholar
Inzlicht, M. and Kang, S. K. (2010). Stereotype threat spillover: How coping with threats to social identity affects aggression, eating, decision making, and attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(3):467–81.Google Scholar
Itti, L., Rees, G., and Tsotsos, J. K. (2005). Neurobiology of attention. Elsevier, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Jackson, F. (1982). Epiphenomenal qualia. Philosophical Quarterly, 32:127–36.Google Scholar
Jackson, F. (1986). What Mary didn’t know. The Journal of Philosophy, 83(5):291–5.Google Scholar
Jackson, S. A. (1996). Toward a conceptual understanding of the flow experience in elite athletes. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67(1):7690.Google Scholar
James, C. A. (1998). Irrationality in philosophy and psychology: The moral implications of self-defeating behaviour. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5(2):224–34.Google Scholar
James, W. (1981). The principles of psychology. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
James, W. (2010). The principles of psychology. H. Holt and company, London.Google Scholar
Jarbo, K. and Verstynen, T. D. (2015). Converging structural and functional connectivity of orbitofrontal, dorsolateral prefrontal, and posterior parietal cortex in the human striatum. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(9):3865–78.Google Scholar
Jaynes, J. (1976). The origin of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. Mariner Books, Boston.Google Scholar
Jaynes, J. (1986). Consciousness and the voices of the mind. Canadian Psychology, 27(2):128–48.Google Scholar
Jenni, N. L., Larkin, J. D., and Floresco, S. B. (2017). Prefrontal dopamine D1 and D2 receptors regulate dissociable aspects of decision-making via distinct ventral striatal and amygdalar circuits. Journal of Neuroscience, 0030–17.Google Scholar
Jennings, C. D. (2012). The subject of attention. Synthese, 189(3):535–54.Google Scholar
Jennings, C. D. (2014). Attention by Wayne Wu. Notre Dame Philosophical Reviews. Retrieved from https://philpapers.org/rec/JENABW.Google Scholar
Jennings, C. D. ( 2015a ). Attention and perceptual organization. Philosophical Studies, 172(5): 1265–78.Google Scholar
Jennings, C. D. ( 2015b ). Consciousness without attention. Journal of the American Philosophical Association, 1(2):276–95.Google Scholar
Jennings, C. D. ( 2015c ). The standard theory of conscious perception. In Noelle, D. C., Dale, R., Warlaumont, A. S., Yoshimi, J., Matlock, T., Jennings, C. D., and Maglio, P. P., editors, Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, pages 986–91. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Jennings, C. D. (2017). I attend, therefore I am: You are only as strong as your powers of attention, and other uncomfortable truths about the self. Aeon Media Group. Retrieved from https://aeon.co/essays/what-is-the-self-if-not-that-which-pays-attention.Google Scholar
Jennings, C. D. (in press). Too much attention, too little self? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research.Google Scholar
Jennings, C. D. and Nanay, B. (2016). Action without attention. Analysis, 76(1): 2936.Google Scholar
Jha, G. (1942). The Chāndogyopanishad (A treatise on Vedānta philosophy translated into English with The Commentary of S’ankara). Oriental Book Agency, Poona.Google Scholar
Jia, X. and Kohn, A. (2011). Gamma rhythms in the brain. PLoS Biology, 9(4):e1001045. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001045.Google Scholar
Jiang, Y., Costello, P., Fang, F., Huang, M., and He, S. (2006). A gender and sexual orientation-dependent spatial attentional effect of invisible images. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 103(45):17048–52.Google Scholar
Johnson, D., Epstein, J., Waid, L., Latham, P., Voronin, K., and Anton, R. (2001). Neuropsychological performance deficits in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 16(6):587604.Google Scholar
Jonas Vidovic, P. (2012). Neuro-cognitive impairments and the criminal justice system: A case analysis of the impact of diagnoses of FASD and ADHD on the sentencing of offenders in the courts of three Canadian provinces (doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from http://summit.sfu.ca/item/12312.Google Scholar
Jonides, J. and Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43(4):346–54.Google Scholar
Jung, K., Ruthruff, E., and Gaspelin, N. (2013). Automatic identification of familiar faces. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75(7):1438–50.Google Scholar
Kaas, J. H., Nelson, R. J., Sur, M., Lin, C.-S., and Merzenich, M. M. (1979). Multiple representations of the body within the primary somatosensory cortex of primates. Science, 204(4392):521–3.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall, Inc., Englewood Cliffs, NJ.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux, New York.Google Scholar
Kanai, R., Tsuchiya, N., and Verstraten, F. A. J. (2006). The scope and limits of top-down attention in unconscious visual processing. Current Biology, 16(23):2332–6.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1914). Critique of judgment (J. H. Bernard, Trans.). Macmillan & Co., Indianapolis.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (1922). Kritik der Urteilskraft. Felix Meiner, Leipzig.Google Scholar
Kant, I. (2003). Critique of pure reason (N. K. Smith, Trans.). Springer, London.Google Scholar
Keller, A. (2016). Mechanisms of consciousness. In Philosophy of olfactory perception, pages 153–65. Springer International Publishing, New York.Google Scholar
Kelso, J. S. (2016). On the self-organizing origins of agency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(7):490–99.Google Scholar
Kenny, A. (1971). The homunculus fallacy. In Grene, M. G. and Prigogine, I., editors, Interpretations of life and mind, pages 155–65. Humanities Press, New York.Google Scholar
Kentridge, R. W. (2013). Visual attention: Bringing the unseen past into view. Current Biology, 23(2):R69–71.Google Scholar
Kilgard, M. P. and Merzenich, M. M. (1995). Anticipated stimuli across skin. Nature, 373(6516):663.Google Scholar
Kim, J. (2007). Physicalism, or something near enough. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.Google Scholar
Kim, J. (2009). Mental causation. In McLaughlin, B., Beckermann, A., and Walter, S., editors, The Oxford handbook of philosophy of mind, pages 2952. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Kim, M. S. and Cave, K. R. (2001). Perceptual grouping via spatial selection in a focused-attention task. Vision Research, 41(5):611–24.Google Scholar
Kinchla, R. and Wolfe, J. (1979). The order of visual processing: “top-down,” “bottom-up,” or “middle-out.Perception & Psychophysics, 25(3):225–31.Google Scholar
King, M. (2009). The problem with negligence. Social Theory and Practice, 35(4):577–95.Google Scholar
Koch, C. and Tsuchiya, N. (2007). Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1):1622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koralus, P. (2014a). Attention, consciousness, and the semantics of questions. Synthese, 191(2):187211.Google Scholar
Koralus, P. ( 2014b ). The erotetic theory of attention: Questions, focus and distraction. Mind & Language, 29(1):2650.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. (1980). Image and mind. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Kouider, S., De Gardelle, V., Sackur, J., and Dupoux, E. (2010). How rich is consciousness? The partial awareness hypothesis. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7):301–7.Google Scholar
Krug, M. and Carter, C. S. (2012). Conflict control loop theory of cognitive control. In Mangun, G. R., editor, The neuroscience of attention: Attentional control and selection, pages 229–49. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
LaBerge, D. (1995). Attentional processing: The brain’s art of mindfulness. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Lakatos, P., Karmos, G., Mehta, A. D., Ulbert, I., and Schroeder, C. E. (2008). Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science, 320(5872):110–13.Google Scholar
Lakatos, P., Shah, A. S., Knuth, K. H., Ulbert, I., Karmos, G., and Schroeder, C. E. (2005). An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94(3):1904–11.Google Scholar
Lamy, D., Tsal, Y., and Egeth, H. E. (2003). Does a salient distractor capture attention early in processing? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3):621–9.Google Scholar
Langer, M. M. (1989). Merleau-Ponty’s Phenomenology of Perception: A guide and commentary. Springer, London.Google Scholar
Lankinen, K., Saari, J., Hlushchuk, Y., Tikka, P., Parkkonen, L., Hari, R., and Koskinen, M. (2018). Consistency and similarity of MEG and fMRI-signal time courses during movie viewing. NeuroImage, 173:361–9.Google Scholar
Lapoujade, D. (2005). The normal and the pathological in Bergson. Modern Language Notes, 120(5):1146–55.Google Scholar
Laureys, S. (2005). The neural correlate of (un) awareness: Lessons from the vegetative state. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12):556–9.Google Scholar
Lee, T. G. and Grafton, S. T. (2015). Out of control: Diminished prefrontal activity coincides with impaired motor performance due to choking under pressure. Neuroimage, 105:145–55.Google Scholar
Leonard, C. J., Robinson, B. M., Hahn, B., Luck, S. J., and Gold, J. M. (2017). Altered spatial profile of distraction in people with schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(8):1077–86.Google Scholar
Levin, D., Takarae, Y., Miner, A., and Keil, F. (2001). Efficient visual search by category: Specifying the features that mark the difference between artifacts and animals in preattentive vision. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(4):676–97.Google Scholar
Levine, J. (1983). Materialism and qualia: The explanatory gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 64(October):354–61.Google Scholar
Lewis, D. (1979). Counterfactual dependence and time’s arrow. Noûs, 13(4):455–76Google Scholar
Li, F., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., and Perona, P. (2003). Natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention: Further explorations. Journal of Vision, 3(9):331. http://journalofvision.org/3/9/331/.doi:10.1167/3.9.331.Google Scholar
Li, F. F., VanRullen, R., Koch, C., and Perona, P. (2002). Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(14):9596–601.Google Scholar
Limb, C. J. and Braun, A. R. (2008). Neural substrates of spontaneous musical performance: An fMRI study of jazz improvisation. PLoS One, 3(2):e1679.Google Scholar
Liu, S., Chow, H. M., Xu, Y., Erkkinen, M. G., Swett, K. E., Eagle, M. W., Rizik-Baer, D. A., and Braun, A. R. (2012). Neural correlates of lyrical improvisation: An fMRI study of freestyle rap. Scientific Reports, 2:834.Google Scholar
Liu, X., Li, J., Gao, J., Zhou, Z., Meng, F., Pan, G., and Luo, B. (2017). Association of medial prefrontal cortex connectivity with consciousness level and its outcome in patients with acquired brain injury. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 42:160–6.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1841). An essay concerning human understanding. Thomas Tegg, London.Google Scholar
Locke, J. (2004). An essay concerning human understanding, volume 1–2. Kessinger Publishing, Whitefish, MT.Google Scholar
Lyyra, P., Mäkelä, H., Hietanen, J. K., and Astikainen, P. (2014). Implicit binding of facial features during change blindness. PLoS One, 9(1):e87682. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0087682.Google Scholar
Mack, A., Erol, M., Clarke, J., and Bert, J. (2016). No iconic memory without attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 40:18.Google Scholar
Mack, A. and Rock, I. (1998a). Inattentional blindness. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Mack, A. and Rock, I.( 1998b). Inattentional blindness: Perception without attention. In Wright, R. D., editor, Visual attention, volume 8, pages 55–76. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
MacLean, K. A., Ferrer, E., Aichele, S. R., et al. (2010). Intensive meditation training improves perceptual discrimination and sustained attention. Psychological Science, 21(6):829–39.Google Scholar
Macnamara, C. (2011). Holding others responsible. Philosophical Studies, 152(1): 81102.Google Scholar
Magee, B. (2001). Talking philosophy: Dialogues with fifteen leading philosophers. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Manly, T., Anderson, V., Nimmo-Smith, I., Turner, A., Watson, P., and Robertson, I. H. (2001). The differential assessment of children’s attention: The test of everyday attention for children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(8):1065–81.Google Scholar
Marshall, G. D. (1970). Attention and will. The Philosophical Quarterly, 20(78): 1425.Google Scholar
Marshall, J. C. and Halligan, P. W. (1988). Blindsight and insight in visuo-spatial neglect. Nature, 336(6201):766–7.Google Scholar
Marti, S. and Dehaene, S. (2017). Discrete and continuous mechanisms of temporal selection in rapid visual streams. Nature Communications, 8(1): 113.Google Scholar
Matthews, J., Schröder, P., Kaunitz, L., van Boxtel, J. J., and Tsuchiya, N. (2018). Conscious access in the near absence of attention: Critical extensions on the dual-task paradigm. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1755):20170352.Google Scholar
McKinley, J. (2000). Amateurs say easy throws can be the hardest. New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2000/06/17/sports/baseball-amateurs-say-easy-throws-can-be-the-hardest.html.Google Scholar
McLaughlin, B. P. (1992). The rise and fall of British emergentism. In Beckermann, A., Flohr, H., and Kim, J., editors, Emergence or reduction? Essays on the prospects for nonreductive physicalism, pages 4994. de Gruyter, Berlin.Google Scholar
McMahon, C. (2008). The prehistory of the concept of attention (Doctoral dissertation). University College Dublin. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000333.Google Scholar
McNamee, D., Liljeholm, M., Zika, O., and O’Doherty, J. P. (2015). Characterizing the associative content of brain structures involved in habitual and goal-directed actions in humans: A multivariate fMRI study. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(9):3764–71.Google Scholar
Mele, A. R. and Moser, P. K. (1994). Intentional action. Noûs, 28(1):3968.Google Scholar
Merickle, P. M. and Joordens, S. (1997). Parallels between perception without attention and perception without awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 6(2–3):219–36.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (2012). Phenomenology of perception (D. A. Landes, Trans.). Routledge, London. (Original work “Phénoménologie de la perception” published 1945.)Google Scholar
Merritt, M. and Valaris, M. (2017). Attention and synthesis in Kant’s conception of experience. The Philosophical Quarterly, 67(268):571–92.Google Scholar
Meter, W. V. (2018). I’m a survivor. New York Magazine. Retrieved from http://nymag.com/nymetro/arts/music/15528/.Google Scholar
Metzinger, T. (2004). Being no one: The self-model theory of subjectivity. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Metzinger, T. (2010). The ego tunnel: The science of the mind and the myth of the self, volume 16. ReadHowYouWant.com. Retrieved from https://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=P1X5FqFBIKwC&oi=fnd&pg=PR3&ots=ankr70Bha8&sig=Z2ZhhWJxVJ_olUiCXDyBIYGgwqM#v=onepage&q&f=false.Google Scholar
Miller, E. K. and Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24(1):167202.Google Scholar
Millikan, R. G. (1989). Biosemantics. The Journal of Philosophy, 86(6):281297.Google Scholar
Mole, C. (2008). Attention and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(4):86104.Google Scholar
Mole, C. ( 2011a ). Attention is cognitive unison: An essay in philosophical psychology. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Mole, C. ( 2011b ). The metaphysics of attention. In Mole, C., Smithies, D., and Wu, W., editors, Attention: Philosophical and psychological essays, pages 6077. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Mole, C. (2017). Attention. In Zalta, E. N., editor, The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford University, Fall 2017 edition. Retrieved from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2017/entries/attention/.Google Scholar
Monchi, O., Petrides, M., Petre, V., Worsley, K., and Dagher, A. (2001). Wisconsin card sorting revisited: Distinct neural circuits participating in different stages of the task identified by event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(19):7733–41.Google Scholar
Montague, M. (2009). The content of perceptual experience. In McLaughlin, B. and Beckermann, A., editors, Oxford handbook of philosophy of mind, pages 494511. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Montemayor, C. and Haladjian, H. H. (2015). Consciousness, attention, and conscious attention. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Montemayor, C. and Haladjian, H. H. (2017). Perception and cognition are largely independent, but still affect each other in systematic ways: Arguments from evolution and the consciousness-attention dissociation. Frontiers in Psychology, 8:40.Google Scholar
Montero, B. (2013). A dancer reflects. In Schear, J. K., editor, Mind, reason and being-in-the-world: The McDowell-Dreyfus debate, pages 303–19. Routledge, London.Google Scholar
Moore, M. S. and Hurd, H. M. (2011). Punishing the awkward, the stupid, the weak, and the selfish: The culpability of negligence. Criminal Law and Philosophy, 5(2):147–98.Google Scholar
Moore, T. and Zirnsak, M. (2017). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Psychology, 68(1):4772.Google Scholar
Moore, W. E. and Stevenson, J. R. (1991). Understanding trust in the performance of complex automatic sport skills. Sport Psychologist, 5(3):281–9.Google Scholar
Mozer, M. C. and Sitton, M. (1998). Computational modeling of spatial attention. In Pashler, H. E., editor, Attention, pages 341–93. Psychology Press, London.Google Scholar
Müller, F. H. (Trans.). (1897). The Upanishads. The Christian Literature Company.Google Scholar
Myers, J. (2017). Does phenomenal consciousness overflow attention? An argument from feature-integration. Florida Philosophical Review, 17(1):2844.Google Scholar
Mylopoulos, M. and Pacherie, E. (2019). Intentions: The dynamic hierarchical model revisited. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 10(2):e1481. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1481.Google Scholar
Nagahama, Y., Okada, T., Katsumi, Y., Hayashi, T., Yamauchi, H., Oyanagi, C., Konishi, J., Fukuyama, H., and Shibasaki, H. (2001). Dissociable mechanisms of attentional control within the human prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 11(1):8592.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? The Philosophical Review, 83(4):435–50.Google Scholar
Nanay, B. (2009). Attention and perceptual content. Analysis, 70(2):263–70.Google Scholar
Navalpakkam, V. and Itti, L. (2006). Top–down attention selection is fine grained. Journal of Vision, 6(11):4.Google Scholar
Nejati, V., Salehinejad, M. A., and Nitsche, M. A. (2018). Interaction of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (l-DLPFC) and right orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) in hot and cold executive functions: Evidence from transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuroscience, 369:109–23.Google Scholar
Neumann, O. (1987). Beyond capacity: A functional view of attention. In Heuer, H. and Sanders, A. F., editors, Perspectives on perception and action, pages 361–94. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
Newsome, W. T. and Pare, E. B. (1988). A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT). Journal of Neuroscience, 8(6):2201–11.Google Scholar
Newton, I. (2010). Descartes, space and body. De gravitatione. (Original work published 1672.) Retrieved from www.earlymoderntexts.com.Google Scholar
Nieoullon, A. (2002). Dopamine and the regulation of cognition and attention. Progress in Neurobiology, 67(1):5383.Google Scholar
Nir, Y. and Tononi, G. (2010). Dreaming and the brain: From phenomenology to neurophysiology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2):88100.Google Scholar
Noë, A. and O’Regan, J. K. (2000). Perception, attention, and the grand illusion. Psyche, 6(15):615.Google Scholar
Nolan, L. (2017). Malebranche’s theory of ideas and vision in God. In Zalta, E. N., editor, The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford University, Spring 2017 edition. Retrieved from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2017/entries/malebranche-ideas/.Google Scholar
Norman, D. and Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In Davidson, R. J., Schwarz, G. E., and Shapiro, D., editors, Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory, volume 4, pages 118. Plenum Publishing Corporation, New York.Google Scholar
Norris, C. (2000). Quantum nonlocality and the challenge to scientific realism. Foundations of Science, 5(1):345.Google Scholar
Noudoost, B. and Moore, T. (2011). The role of neuromodulators in selective attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(12):585–91.Google Scholar
Nunez, P. L. and Srinivasan, R. (2006). A theoretical basis for standing and traveling brain waves measured with human EEG with implications for an integrated consciousness. Clinical Neurophysiology, 117(11):2424–35.Google Scholar
O’Connor, T. and Wong, H. Y. (2015). Emergent properties. In Zalta, E. N., editor, The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford University, Summer 2015 edition. Retrieved from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2015/entries/properties-emergent/.Google Scholar
O’Craven, K. (2005). Neural evidence for object-based attention. In Itti, L., Rees, G., and Tsotsos, J. K., editors, Neurobiology of attention. Elsevier Academic Press, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Ogawa, H. (1994). Gustatory cortex of primates: Anatomy and physiology. Neuroscience Research, 20(1):113.Google Scholar
Ogawa, T. and Komatsu, H. (2004). Target selection in area V4 during a multidimensional visual search task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24(28): 6371–82.Google Scholar
Oliva, A. (2005). Gist of the scene. In Neurobiology of attention, pages 251–7. Elsevier Academic Press, Amsterdam.Google Scholar
Oliva, A. and Torralba, A. (2006). Building the gist of a scene: The role of global image features in recognition. Progress in Brain Research, 155:2336.Google Scholar
Olivelle, P. (Trans.). (2008). The Upanishads. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Olsen, G. D. (1995). Creating the contrast: The influence of silence and background music on recall and attribute importance. Journal of Advertising, 24(4):2944.Google Scholar
Orlandi, N. (2013). Embedded seeing: Vision in the natural world. Noûs, 47(4):727–47.Google Scholar
Orosz, P. (2010). Ayrton Senna on the very edge. Jalopnik. Retrieved from http://jalopnik.com/5452870/ayrton-senna-on-the-very-edge.Google Scholar
Papineau, D. (2009). The causal closure of the physical and naturalism. In McLaughlin, B., Beckermann, A., and Walter, S., editors, The Oxford handbook of philosophy of mind, pages 5365. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Papineau, D. (2015). Choking and the yips. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 14(2):295308.Google Scholar
Parasuraman, R. (1998). The attentive brain. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Pashler, H. E. (1998). The psychology of attention. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Pavlopoulou, C. and Yu, S. (2010). Classification and feature selection with human performance data. In IEEE International Conference on Image Processing, pages 1557–60. IEEE.Google Scholar
Penfield, W. and Perot, P. (1963). The brain’s record of auditory and visual experience: A final summary and discussion. Brain, 86(4):595696.Google Scholar
People v. Bond (2012). People v. Bond, No. C065733 (Cal. Ct. App. June 25, 2012).Google Scholar
Peppers-Bates, S. (2005). Does Malebranche need efficacious ideas? The cognitive faculties, the ontological status of ideas, and human attention. Journal of the History of Philosophy, 43(1):83105.Google Scholar
Petersen, S. E. and Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35:7389.Google Scholar
Phillips, I. B. (2011). Perception and iconic memory: What Sperling doesn’t show. Mind & Language, 26(4):381411.Google Scholar
Pineda, D., Ardila, A., and Rosselli, M. (1999). Neuropsychological and behavioral assessment of ADHD in seven to twelve-year-old children: A discriminant analysis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32(2):159–73.Google Scholar
Poldrack, R., Sabb, F., Foerde, K., Tom, S., Asarnow, R., Bookheimer, S., and Knowlton, B. (2005). The neural correlates of motor skill automaticity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(22):5356–64.Google Scholar
Posner, M. (2012). Attentional networks and consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 3:14.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1):325.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 91:7398–403.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I., DiGirolamo, G. J., and Fernandez-Duque, D. (1997). Brain mechanisms of cognitive skills. Consciousness and Cognition, 6:267–90.Google Scholar
Posner, M. I. and Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13(1):2542.Google Scholar
Postle, B. R. (2006). Working memory as an emergent property of the mind and brain. Neuroscience, 139(1):2338.Google Scholar
Potter, M. (1976). Short-term conceptual memory for pictures. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 2(5):509–29.Google Scholar
Association, Press (2015). Boy, 14, killed man with one punch after giving him directions. Guardian. Retrieved from www.theguardian.com/uk-news/2015/apr/10/14-year-old-boy-killed-man-with-one-punch-after-giving-him-directions.Google Scholar
Prettyman, A. (2017). Perceptual content is indexed to attention. Synthese, 194(10):4039–54.Google Scholar
Priftis, K., Schiff, S., Tikhonoff, V., Giordano, N., Amodio, P., Umiltà, C., and Casiglia, E. (2011). Hypnosis meets neuropsychology: Simulating visuospatial neglect in healthy participants. Neuropsychologia, 49(12):3346–50.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2011a). Is attention necessary and sufficient for consciousness? In Mole, C., Smithies, D., and Wu, W., editors, Attention: Philosophical and psychological essays, pages 174203. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. ( 2011b ). Waiting for the self. In Liu, J. and Perry, J., editors, Consciousness and the self: New essays, pages 123–49. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511732355.006.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2012). The conscious brain. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Prochazka, A., Clarac, F., Loeb, G. E., Rothwell, J. C., and Wolpaw, J. R. (2000). What do reflex and voluntary mean? Modern views on an ancient debate. Experimental Brain Research, 130(4):417–32.Google Scholar
Proust, J. (2013). Mental acts as natural kinds. In Clark, A., Kiverstein, J., Vierkant, T., and Vierkant, T., editors, Decomposing the will, pages 262–82. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Puttemans, V., Wenderoth, N., and Swinnen, S. P. (2005). Changes in brain activation during the acquisition of a multifrequency bimanual coordination task: From the cognitive stage to advanced levels of automaticity. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25(17):4270–8.Google Scholar
Pyle, A. (2013). Locke. John Wiley & Sons, Cambridge.Google Scholar
R. v. L.A.B. (2007). R. v. L.A.B., 2007 ONCJ 538.Google Scholar
R. v. McGoran (2004). R. v. McGoran, 2004 BCSC 1737.Google Scholar
Raffman, D. (1993). Language, music, and mind. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Ratliff, B. (1999). Pop review; performance anxiety: Hiding onstage. New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/1999/01/05/arts/pop-review-performance-anxiety-hiding-onstage.html.Google Scholar
Rayno, A. (2011). Chuck Knoblauch: Unpacking memories. Star Tribune. Retrieved from www.startribune.com/about-that-chuck-knoblauch-story/62651622/.Google Scholar
Raz, J. (2010). Responsibility and the negligence standard. Oxford Journal of Legal Studies, 30(1):118.Google Scholar
Reddy, L., Reddy, L., and Koch, C. (2006). Face identification in the near-absence of focal attention. Vision Research, 46(15):2336–43.Google Scholar
Reddy, L., Wilken, P., and Koch, C. (2004). Face-gender discrimination is possible in the near-absence of attention. Journal of Vision, 4(2):106–17.Google Scholar
Rensink, R. A. (2015). A function-centered taxonomy of visual attention. In Coates, P. and Coleman, S., editors, Phenomenal qualities: Sense, perception, and consciousness, pages 347–75. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., and Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8:368–73.Google Scholar
Reynolds, J. H. and Heeger, D. J. (2009). The normalization model of attention. Neuron, 61(2):168–85.Google Scholar
Ro, T. and Rafal, R. (1996). Perception of geometric illusions in hemispatial neglect. Neuropsychologia, 34(10):973–8.Google Scholar
Robertson, L. C. (2003). Binding, spatial attention and perceptual awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(2):93102.Google Scholar
Rosenbaum, D. A., Carlson, R. A., and Gilmore, R. O. (2001). Acquisition of intellectual and perceptual-motor skills. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1):453–70.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, M., Finn, E., Scheinost, D., Constable, R., and Chun, M. (2017). Characterizing attention with predictive network models. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(4):290302.Google Scholar
Rosenberg, M. D., Finn, E. S., Scheinost, D., Papademetris, X., Shen, X., Constable, R. T., and Chun, M. M. (2016). A neuromarker of sustained attention from whole-brain functional connectivity. Nature Neuroscience, 19(1):165–71.Google Scholar
Rosenblum, L. D., Schmuckler, M. A., and Johnson, J. A. (1997). The McGurk effect in infants. Perception & Psychophysics, 59(3):347–57.Google Scholar
Rosenthal, D. M. (2002). Explaining consciousness. In Chalmers, D. J., editor, Philosophy of mind: Classical and contemporary readings, pages 406–21. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Rossi, A. F., Pessoa, L., Desimone, R., and Ungerleider, L. G. (2009). The prefrontal cortex and the executive control of attention. Experimental Brain Research, 192(3):489–97.Google Scholar
Russell, B. (2009). Human knowledge: Its scope and its limits. Routledge Classics, London.Google Scholar
Saalmann, Y. B., Pigarev, I. N., and Vidyasagar, T. R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of visual attention: How top-down feedback highlights relevant locations. Science, 316(5831):1612–15.Google Scholar
Salam, M. and Victor, D. (2018). Yanny or Laurel? How a sound clip divided America. New York Times. Retrieved from www.nytimes.com/2018/05/15/science/yanny-laurel.html.Google Scholar
Saling, L. L. and Phillips, J. (2007). Automatic behaviour: Efficient not mindless. Brain Research Bulletin, 73(1):120.Google Scholar
Saran, K. (2018). Does Mole’s argument that cognitive processes fail to suffice for attention fail? Ergo, 5(18):487505. http://dx.doi.org/10.3998/ergo.12405314.0005.018.Google Scholar
Sarter, M., Givens, B., and Bruno, J. P. (2001). The cognitive neuroscience of sustained attention: Where top-down meets bottom-up. Brain Research Reviews, 35(2):146–60.Google Scholar
Schmader, T., Johns, M., and Forbes, C. (2008). An integrated process model of stereotype threat effects on performance. Psychological Review, 115(2):336–56.Google Scholar
Schneider, K. A. (2011). Attention alters decision criteria but not appearance: A reanalysis of Anton-Erxleben, Abrams, and Carrasco (2010). Journal of Vision, 11(13):18.Google Scholar
Schneider, W. and Chein, J. M. (2003). Controlled & automatic processing: Behavior, theory, and biological mechanisms. Cognitive Science, 27(3):525–59.Google Scholar
Schneider, W., Pimm-Smith, M., and Worden, M. (1994). Neurobiology of attention and automaticity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 4(2):177–82.Google Scholar
Schroeder, C. E. and Lakatos, P. (2009). Low-frequency neuronal oscillations as instruments of sensory selection. Trends in Neurosciences, 32(1):918.Google Scholar
Schroeter, F. (2004). Endorsement and autonomous agency. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 69(3):633–59.Google Scholar
Schwartz, L. (2015). A brooding genius. ESPN. Retrieved from https://espn.go.com/sportscentury/features/00016440.html.Google Scholar
Schwitzgebel, E. (2007). Do you have constant tactile experience of your feet in your shoes? Or is experience limited to what’s in attention? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14(3):535.Google Scholar
Schyns, P. and Oliva, A. (1994). From blobs to boundary edges: Evidence for time-and spatial-scale-dependent scene recognition. Psychological Science, 5(4):195200.Google Scholar
Schyns, P. and Oliva, A. (1999). Dr. Angry and Mr. Smile: When categorization flexibly modifies the perception of faces in rapid visual presentations. Cognition, 69(3):243–65.Google Scholar
Seegmiller, J. K., Watson, J. M., and Strayer, D. L. (2011). Individual differences in susceptibility to inattentional blindness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(3):785–91.Google Scholar
Seger, C. A. and Cincotta, C. M. (2005). Dynamics of frontal, striatal, and hippocampal systems during rule learning. Cerebral Cortex, 16(11):1546–55.Google Scholar
Seitz, A. R. and Watanabe, T. (2008). Is task-irrelevant learning really task-irrelevant? PLoS One, 3(11):e3792. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0003792.Google Scholar
Seitz, A. R. and Watanabe, T. (2009). The phenomenon of task-irrelevant perceptual learning. Vision Research, 49(21):2604–10.Google Scholar
Sellars, W. (1997). Empiricism and the philosophy of mind, volume 1. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Sendiña-Nadal, I., Buldú, J. M., Leyva, I., and Boccaletti, S. (2008). Phase locking induces scale-free topologies in networks of coupled oscillators. PLoS One, 3(7):e2644. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0002644.Google Scholar
Shannon, C. E. (1948). A mathematical theory of communication. Bell System Technical Journal, 27:379423.Google Scholar
Sharma, M., Resta, R., and Car, R. (2007). Dipolar correlations and the dielectric permittivity of water. Physical Review Letters, 98(24):247401. https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevLett.98.247401.Google Scholar
Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., and Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The interplay between goal intentions and implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(1):8798.Google Scholar
Shepherd, J. (2014). The contours of control. Philosophical Studies, 170(3):395411.Google Scholar
Shepherd, J. (2015). Conscious control over action. Mind & Language, 30(3):320–44.Google Scholar
Shepherd, J. (2019). Skilled action and the double life of intention. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 98(2):286305.Google Scholar
Sheredos, B. (2017). Act psychology and phenomenology: Husserl on egoic acts. Husserl Studies, 33(3):191209.Google Scholar
Sider, T. (2003). What’s so bad about overdetermination? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 67(3):719–26.Google Scholar
Siegel, S. and Silins, N. (2015). The epistemology of perception. In Matthen, M., editor, Oxford handbook of philosophy of perception, pages 781812. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Simons, D. J. (2000). Attentional capture and inattentional blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(4):147–55.Google Scholar
Simons, D. J. and Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28:1059–74.Google Scholar
Singer, W. and Gray, C. M. (1995). Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18:555–86.Google Scholar
Sirotin, Y. B. and Das, A. (2009). Anticipatory haemodynamic signals in sensory cortex not predicted by local neuronal activity. Nature, 457(7228):475–9.Google Scholar
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. Simon and Schuster, New York.Google Scholar
Sklar, R. H. (2013). Hyperfocus in adult ADHD: An EEG study of the differences in cortical activity in resting and arousal states (master’s thesis). Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/10210/8640.Google Scholar
Smith, A. M. (2005). Responsibility for attitudes: Activity and passivity in mental life. Ethics, 115(2):236–71.Google Scholar
Smith, K. S., Virkud, A., Deisseroth, K., and Graybiel, A. M. (2012). Reversible online control of habitual behavior by optogenetic perturbation of medial prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(46):18932–7.Google Scholar
Smithies, D. (2011). Attention is rational-access consciousness. In Mole, C., Smithies, D., and Wu, W., editors, Attention: Philosophical and psychological essays, pages 247–73. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Souter, A. (1968). Oxford Latin dictionary. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Spelke, E., Hirst, W., and Neisser, U. (1976). Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4(3):215–30.Google Scholar
Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs, 74(11):129.Google Scholar
Spratling, M. W. and Johnson, M. H. (2004). A feedback model of visual attention. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(2):219–37.Google Scholar
State v. Scott (2001). State v. Scott, 91 Ohio St.3d 1268.Google Scholar
Straight, S. (1977). Consciousness as a workspace. SISTEM Quarterly, 1:1114.Google Scholar
Strawson, G. (2009). The self. In McLaughlin, B., Beckermann, A., and Walter, S., editors, The Oxford handbook of philosophy of mind, pages 541–64. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Strawson, G. (2017). The subject of experience. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Suchy-Dicey, C. (2012). Inductive parsimony and the methodological argument. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(2):605–9.Google Scholar
Suchy-Dicey, C. and Watanabe, T. (2010). Re-thinking the active-passive distinction in attention from a philosophical viewpoint. Journal of Vision, 10(7):218.Google Scholar
Suhler, C. L. and Churchland, P. S. (2009). Control: Conscious and otherwise. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8):341–7.Google Scholar
Szczepanski, S. M. and Knight, R. T. (2014). Insights into human behavior from lesions to the prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 83(5):1002–18.Google Scholar
Talavage, T. M., Sereno, M. I., Melcher, J. R., Ledden, P. J., Rosen, B. R., and Dale, A. M. (2004). Tonotopic organization in human auditory cortex revealed by progressions of frequency sensitivity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91(3):1282–96.Google Scholar
Tallon-Baudry, C. (2012). On the neural mechanisms subserving consciousness and attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 2:111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00397.Google Scholar
Tallon-Baudry, C., Campana, F., Park, H.-D., and Babo-Rebelo, M. (2018). The neural monitoring of visceral inputs, rather than attention, accounts for first-person perspective in conscious vision. Cortex, 102:139–49.Google Scholar
Taylor, J. H. (2015). Against unifying accounts of attention. Erkenntnis, 80(1): 3956.Google Scholar
Terwagne, D. and Bush, J. W. (2011). Tibetan singing bowls. Nonlinearity, 24(8):R51–66.Google Scholar
Thakur, K. and Ropper, A. (2011). Transient global amnesia during a professional cello concert. Journal of Clinical Neuroscience, 18(9):1260–1.Google Scholar
Thompson, E. (2014). Waking, dreaming, being: Self and consciousness in neuroscience, meditation, and philosophy. Columbia University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Thrailkill, E. A. and Bouton, M. E. (2015). Contextual control of instrumental actions and habits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 41(1):6980.Google Scholar
Toadvine, T. (2018). Maurice Merleau-Ponty. In Zalta, E. N., editor, The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford University, Spring 2018 edition. Retrieved from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2019/entries/merleau-ponty/.Google Scholar
Tononi, G. (2005). Consciousness, information integration, and the brain. Progress in Brain Research, 105:109–26.Google Scholar
Tononi, G., Boly, M., Massimini, M., and Koch, C. (2016). Integrated information theory: From consciousness to its physical substrate. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(7):450–61.Google Scholar
Tootell, R. B., Hadjikhani, N. K., Vanduffel, W., Liu, A. K., Mendola, J. D., Sereno, M. I., and Dale, A. M. (1998). Functional analysis of primary visual cortex (V1) in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95(3):811–17.Google Scholar
Trantham-Davidson, H., Neely, L. C., Lavin, A., and Seamans, J. K. (2004). Mechanisms underlying differential D1 versus D2 dopamine receptor regulation of inhibition in prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 24(47):10652–9.Google Scholar
Trappenberg, T. P., Dorris, M. C., Munoz, D. P., and Klein, R. M. (2001). A model of saccade initiation based on the competitive integration of exogenous and endogenous signals in the superior colliculus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13(2): 256–71.Google Scholar
Trask, S. and Bouton, M. E. (2014). Contextual control of operant behavior: Evidence for hierarchical associations in instrumental learning. Learning & Behavior, 42(3):281–8.Google Scholar
Travis, S. L., Dux, P. E., and Mattingley, J. B. (2017). Re-examining the influence of attention and consciousness on visual afterimage duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 43(12):1944–9.Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M. (1988). Features and objects: The fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40(2):201–37.Google Scholar
Treisman, A. M. and Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1):97136.Google Scholar
Trübutschek, D., Marti, S., Ojeda, A., King, J.-R., Mi, Y., Tsodyks, M., and Dehaene, S. (2017). A theory of working memory without consciousness or sustained activity. Elife, 6:e23871. DOI:10.7554/eLife.23871.001.Google Scholar
Tsuchiya, N. and Koch, C. (2016). The relationship between consciousness and top-down attention. In Laureys, S., Gosseries, O., and Tononi, G., editors, The neurology of conciousness, pages 71–91. Academic Press, Amsterdam, second edition.Google Scholar
Turcotte, D. L. and Rundle, J. B. (2002). Self-organized complexity in the physical, biological, and social sciences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 99(1):2463–5.Google Scholar
Tzelgov, J. (1997). Specifying the relations between automaticity and consciousness: A theoretical note. Consciousness and Cognition, 6:441–51.Google Scholar
van Boxtel, J. J. (2017). Different signal enhancement pathways of attention and consciousness underlie perception in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(24):5912–22.Google Scholar
van der Heijden, A. H. C. and Bem, S. (1997). Successive approximations to an adequate model of attention. Consciousness and Cognition, 6:413–28.Google Scholar
van Kerkoerle, T., Self, M. W., Dagnino, B., Gariel-Mathis, M., Poort, J., Van Der Togt, C., and Roelfsema, P. R. (2014). Alpha and gamma oscillations characterize feedback and feedforward processing in monkey visual cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(40):14332–41.Google Scholar
van Leeuwen, T. M., Den Ouden, H. E., and Hagoort, P. (2011). Effective connectivity determines the nature of subjective experience in graphemecolor synesthesia. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31(27):9879–84.Google Scholar
Velmans, M. (1995). The relation of consciousness to the material world. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(3):255–65.Google Scholar
Verdun-Jones, S. N. and Butler, A. (2013). Sentencing neurocognitively impaired offenders in Canada. Canadian Journal of Criminology and Criminal Justice, 55(4):495512.Google Scholar
Vermersch, P. (2004). Attention between phenomenology and experimental psychology. Continental Philosophy Review, 37(1):4581.Google Scholar
Wager, T. D., Rilling, J. K., Smith, E. E., Sokolik, A., Casey, K. L., Davidson, R. J., Kosslyn, S. M., Rose, R. M., and Cohen, J. D. (2004). Placeboinduced changes in fMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science, 303(5661):1162–7.Google Scholar
Watson, A. (2018). Contrasting Nyāya-Vaiśeṣika and Buddhist explanations of attention. Philosophy East and West, 68(4):12921313.Google Scholar
Watzl, S. (2010). The significance of attention (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from ProQuest (UMI No: 3447994).Google Scholar
Watzl, S. (2011). Attention as structuring the stream of consciousness. In Mole, C., Smithies, D., and Wu, W., editors, Attention: Philosophical and psychological essays, pages 145–73. Oxford University Press, New York.Google Scholar
Watzl, S. (2017). Structuring mind: The nature of attention and how it shapes consciousness. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Watzl, S. (2018). Consciousness and no self? Ratio, 31(4):363–75.Google Scholar
Webb, T. W. and Graziano, M. S. A. (2015). The attention schema theory: A mechanistic account of subjective awareness. Frontiers in Psychology, 6:500. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00500.Google Scholar
White, R. C. and Davies, A. A. (2008). Attention set for number: Expectation and perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(5):10921107.Google Scholar
Wilding, J. (2005). Is attention impaired in ADHD? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(4):487505.Google Scholar
Wilken, P., Bayne, T. J., and Cleeremans, A. (2009). The Oxford companion to consciousness. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Wilson, C. R., Gaffan, D., Browning, P. G., and Baxter, M. G. (2010). Functional localization within the prefrontal cortex: Missing the forest for the trees? Trends in Neurosciences, 33(12):533–40.Google Scholar
Wilson, G. and Shpall, S. (2012). Action. In Zalta, E. N., editor, The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Stanford University, Summer 2012 edition. Retrieved from https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2016/entries/action/.Google Scholar
Windt, J. M. (2015). Dreaming: A conceptual framework for philosophy of mind and empirical research. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (2009). Philosophical investigations (G. M. Anscombe, P. S. Hacker, and J. Schulte, Trans.). John Wiley and Sons, Oxford.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M. (1998). Visual search. In Pashler, H. E., editor, Attention, pages 1373. Psychology Press, London.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M. (1999). Inattentional amnesia. Journal of Mental Imagery, 29(2–4):7194.Google Scholar
Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., and Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15(3):419–33.Google Scholar
Wollstonecraft, M. (2002). A vindication of the rights of woman, With strictures on political and moral subjects. Gutenberg Books. Retrieved from www.gutenberg.org/ebooks/3420.Google Scholar
Wood, W. and Rünger, D. (2016). Psychology of habit. Annual Review of Psychology, 67:289314.Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2011a). Attention as selection for action. In Mole, C., Smithies, D., and Wu, W., editors, Attention: Philosophical and psychological essays, pages 97116. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Wu, W. ( 2011b ). Confronting many-many problems: Attention and agentive control. Noûs, 45(1):5076.Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2013). Mental action and the threat of automaticity. In Clark, A., Kiverstein, J., Vierkant, T., and Vierkant, T., editors, Decomposing the will, pages 244–61. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2014). Attention. Routledge, London.Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2015). Experts and deviants: The story of agentive control. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 93(1):101–26.Google Scholar
Wu, W. (2018). Action always involves attention. Analysis. https://doi.org/10.1093/analys/any080.Google Scholar
Yaffe, G. (2009). Thomas Reid on consciousness and attention. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 39(2):165–94.Google Scholar
Yamaguchi, S., Yamagata, S., and Kobayashi, S. (2000). Cerebral asymmetry of the top-down allocation of attention to global and local features. Journal of Neuroscience, 20(RC72):1–5.Google Scholar
Yantis, S. (2013). Sensation and perception. Macmillan International Higher Education, New York.Google Scholar
Yantis, S. and Johnson, D. N. (1990). Mechanisms of attentional priority. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16(4):812–25.Google Scholar
Yantis, S. and Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Voluntary versus automatic allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 16(1):121–34.Google Scholar
Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye movements during perception of complex objects. Springer, Boston.Google Scholar
Yin, H. H. and Knowlton, B. J. (2006). The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(6):464–76.Google Scholar
Young, S., Kopelman, M., and Gudjonsson, G. (2009). Forensic neuropsychology in practice: A guide to assessment and legal processes. Oxford University Press, Oxford.Google Scholar
Young, S. J., Adamou, M., Bolea, B., Gudjonsson, G., Müller, U., Pitts, M., Thome, J., and Asherson, P. (2011). The identification and management of ADHD offenders within the criminal justice system: A consensus statement from the UK Adult ADHD Network and criminal justice agencies. BMC Psychiatry, 11(1):114.Google Scholar
Yu, R. (2015). Choking under pressure: The neuropsychological mechanisms of incentive-induced performance decrements. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 9:19.Google Scholar
Yuan, T. (2015). Scaling in stratocumulus fields: An emergent property. arXiv e-prints (1506.08768). Retrieved from https://arxiv.org/abs/1506.08768.Google Scholar
Zanesco, A. P., King, B., MacLean, K., and Saron, C. D. (2013). Executive control and felt concentrative engagement following intensive meditation training. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11(32):113. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00566.Google Scholar
Zanker, J. M. and Abdullah, A. K. (2004). Are size illusions in simple line drawings affected by shading? Perception, 33(12):1475–82.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, M. J. (1986). Negligence and moral responsibility. Noûs, 20(2): 199218.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×